Joaquín
Fuster; Memory in the Cerebral Cortex |
|
|
Book |
Page |
|
Topic |
|
|
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
3 |
|
Perceptual
and motor memory
relationships, and the interplay of their networks, constitute the basis of
the perception-action cycle. |
|
|
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
3 |
|
Entire neocortex as a mnemonic unit. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
4 |
|
Short-term
and long-term memory share much of the same cortical
substrate and simply reflect different activation states of that substrate. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
4 |
|
There is no
such thing as an entirely
new experience or memory. All that is apparently
new to us happens in
the context of old and well-established memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
4 |
|
We interpret the new always in
light of (I.e. in association with) the old. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
4 |
|
Storage of short-term
memories is inextricable from the reactivation of a long-term memories. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
5 |
|
Working memory, which has also been called operant memory, is an operant
concept of active memory,.but the two are not identical. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
9 |
|
Individual versus phyletic memory |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
9 |
|
Phyletic memory, or memory of the species. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
10 |
|
Phyletic memory contains the innate capacity to respond to and to reenact (to
recall) the elementary features of sensation and movement that are common to the repertoires of all members of the species. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
10 |
|
Individual memory is contained in higher cortex, mostly (but not exclusively) in cortex
of association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
10 |
|
Phyletic
and individual memory
probably blend smoothly
both structurally and functionally, from one to the other. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
10 |
|
In an awake organism, there is a
constant dynamic interaction
between phyletic an individual memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
11 |
|
All memory is essentially associative. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
11 |
|
Individual memories are formed by the facilitation, or perhaps creation, of synaptic
connections between neurons that represent
different sensory or motor features, when such features co-occur in the
internal or external environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
11 |
|
Informational content of memory networks resides in the associative
relationships among neuronal
elements. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
Two basic forms of information
content -- perceptual and motor memory. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
Activation state of memory -- a memory trace
(subnetwork) is active or inactive. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
Short-term versus long-term memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
An essential criterion for the distinction between short-term and long-term memory is their temporal persistence -- the length
of time during which it memory is retained and
retrievable.
That length of time may be seconds to
minutes in short-term
memory and up to years in long-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
Information capacity is another distinction between short-term and long-term
memory. The capacity of short-term
memory is limited, whereas that of long-term memory supposedly is not. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
12 |
|
Another difference between
short-term and long-term memory is that the retrieval of
long-term memory is facilitated by specific cues, whereas that of
short-term memory is not. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
13 |
|
Several kinds of short-term memory have been described, mainly on the basis of storage time
distinctions or neuropsychological data. The shortest of short-term memories is iconic memory, which is the capacity to retain a sensory image or up to one second after presentation. Immediate
memory would last a
few seconds longer than iconic memory. It coincides of what is commonly understood as short-term
memory. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
14 |
|
Working memory, a concept of short-term memory is essentially a temporary storage used in
performance of cognitive behavioral tasks. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
16 |
|
It is a reasonable presumption
that all types of short-term memory as well as long-term memory essentially share the same
cortical substrate. The basic differences between them would
lie in the degree and distribution of neuronal activity within the substrate. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
16 |
|
Short-term memory probably, and essentially, involves the sustained activation of a vast cortical network. The activation of a
vast network is the physiological basis of what we can understand to be active memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
16 |
|
Declarative versus
non-declarative memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
20 |
|
Perceptual versus motor memory |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
23 |
|
3 - Principles of neural memory
formation |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
23 |
|
Hebb's postulate |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
26 |
|
Synchronous convergence |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
33 |
|
Synchronous divergence |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
36 |
|
Limbic system and memory
consolidation |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
38 |
|
Cortical connections of the
barrel hippocampal gyrus. (diagram) |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
40 |
|
Neurotransmitters and memory
consolidation |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
4 - Anatomy of cortical memory |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
Basic Architecture of the Cortex |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
Structure of the neocortex is remarkably similar throughout. Horizontal and
vertical regularities in its cells and fibers. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
Architectonic differences between cortical areas are ontogenetically determined by afferent connections. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
Thalamic afferents as the crucial determining factor of cortical functions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
47 |
|
Functional specificity may not be determined only by thalamocortical afferents but by corticocortical
connections as well. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
48 |
|
Lamina organization of the neocortex with its characteristic six layers. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
48 |
|
Recurrent collaterals from pyramids usually follow a horizontal or
oblique course and spread around as much is 2 to 3 mm. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
48 |
|
Arrays of fibers that run
vertically, perpendicular to the cortical surface. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
48 |
|
Apical dendrites of pyramidal cells that group themselves
into bundles
approximately 100 µ thick. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
48 |
|
Corticocortical afferents from afar commonly terminate on apical dendrites in layers I to III. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Vertical plexuses of
thalamic afferents
ascending to layer IV. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Axons from pyramidal
cells in layers III,
V, and VI, descending to the white matter with subcortical or cortical destination. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Geometry of
cortical connectivity
contains, implicitly, the potential for extensive parallel processing along vertical arrays of neural
elements as well as for corticocortical
association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
One of the most significant
characteristics of sensory cortical areas is there a modular organization. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Sensory cortical areas are arranged in columns (50 to 80
µ wide) that traverse the cortex orthogonal to its surface. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
The column, or minicolumn, could be
conceptualized as the ubiquitous and irreducible functional unit of the cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Outside of primary sensory
areas, the modular organization is less clear. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
49 |
|
Cortical pyramidal cell
surrounded by recurrent collaterals.
(diagram) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
50 |
|
Most of the thalami
afferents and some of the cortical afferents to a column
terminate on the neurons of layer IV. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
50 |
|
Most corticocortical
fibers originate and terminate in cells of supragranular layer 3. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
50 |
|
Important outputs to cortical and subcortical
locations emerge from
layers III, V, and VI. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
50 |
|
Corticocortical inputs and
outputs are pervasive in the neocortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
51 |
|
Idealized column of cortex. (diagram) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
PHYLETIC CORTICAL MEMORY |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
At least some cortical
information processing along ontogenetic gradients, from early-maturing
sensory areas to late-maturing areas of association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
Sensory Phyletic Memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
At birth, the primary sensory systems of the
mammalian brain are almost entirely developed anatomically. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
At birth,
the basic architecture
of the adult sensory cortex has been attained, its neural elements are in place, and its connectivity is virtually complete. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
After an overproduction of cortical cells and synapses, both will decrease in numbers
postnatally and eventually will stabilize at adult levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
52 |
|
To achieve its
full functional development, the visual cortex
needs visual experience
during a critical period of early life. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
53 |
|
Arborization of thalamic axons
that reach cortical layer IV. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
53 |
|
Ocular dominance columns |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
53 |
|
Inputs compete in early ontogeny
for development of synapses. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
53 |
|
Sparing of synapses from normal
perinatal attrition -- after overproduction -- depends on the availability of
input. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
54 |
|
Motor phyletic memory |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
56 |
|
Late plasticity of primary areas |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
60 |
|
Sight, sound, and touch |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
67 |
|
Hierarchical processing of visual
information along the
primary visual pathway,
within what could be ascribed to phyletic memory, was a direct inference from the studies of Hubel and Wiesel (1962, 1968). |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
76 |
|
Taste and Smell |
|
9 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
76 |
|
Taste and smell are supposed to be phylogenetically older than the
other senses and, perhaps because of their ancient
origin, have their cortical
representation in phylogenetically
older, paralimbic cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
76 |
|
Gustatory input is collected through the ventromedial posterior (VMP) nucleus
of the thalamus and
conveyed to the primary sensory cortex for taste, which lies in the frontal insula
and operculum, in the depths
of the sylvian
fissures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
76 |
|
Input from the olfactory bulb goes directly to the primary olfactory cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
76 |
|
Gustatory cortex of the insula and operculum has been found to project to the caudolateral area of the orbitofrontal
cortex, at the base of
the frontal lobe. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
Processing of
taste information follows the same general pattern of neural progressions as
that of the other three modalities, but perhaps with fewer cortical stages. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
In general, sensory
processing proceeds
from primary sensory processing, to secondary or associated
processing, and output to limbic structures, which reciprocate with back projections. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
Olfactory information is
transmitted directly from the olfactory bulb to the cortex, and then
processed through thalamus, and then projected to the medial orbitofrontal
area. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
The main difference between
gustatory and olfactory systems seems to be that in the olfactory system, a
thalamic stage succeeds, rather than precedes, primary sensory cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
In olfaction, a thalamic nucleus
is interposed between sensory cortex and the association cortex or the
orbitofrontal region. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
MOTOR CORTICAL HIERARCHY |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
Motor cortical hierarchy goes from association cortex (prefrontal cortex) to motor cortex.Connectivity between the areas is reciprocal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
All three major steps (prefrontal, premotor, and motor) of the frontal motor hierarchy project to subcortical structures and receive reentrant connections from them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
Long reentrant connective loops have been identified that originate in frontal areas, course through the basal ganglia and the lateral thalamus, and return to frontal
areas. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
In accord with a general rule of
corticocortical reciprocity,
frontal areas send projections to all the areas of postcentral cortex from which they do receive them;
and, again, that connectivity appears to be topologically
orderly. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
77 |
|
Prefrontal
and premotor cortices
appear to develop after
primary motor cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
Development
of cortical architecture
progresses from area 4 to 6 and 6 to 9 -- thus from motor to premotor and from there to prefrontal cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
Sensory processing goes from particulars of sensory
analysis to the synthetic
generalities of perception. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
Motor processing goes in the reverse of sensory
processing -- it progresses from generalities of
the temporal structure of behavior, presumably represented in prefrontal
cortex, to the particulars
of movement, to the the microgenesis of the action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
Progression of the processing
from the general to the particular, down the frontal
motor hierarchy, is reflected in the patterns of frontal cell discharge in anticipation of motor acts. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
Prefrontal cells fire with the longest lead time before movement. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
An excitatory
volley successively recruits, in cascading fashion, the three stages of the descending motor hierarchy before the movement. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
78 |
|
At the top
of the motor hierarchy, the prefrontal cortex represents the most global and
temporally extended aspects of behavioral structure. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
80 |
|
Interhemispheric connectivity |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
81 |
|
Individualized anatomy of cortical memory |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
83 |
|
5 - Memory Networks |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
83 |
|
Two fundamental principles of
processing and representation -- convergence for synthesis and divergence for analysis. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
83 |
|
Limbic structures, especially the hippocampus, in the consolidation of cortical memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
83 |
|
Connective substrate of sensory and motor hierarchies for processing and representation of perceptual and motor information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
85 |
|
The most successful models of human memory are those of distributed and self-organized associative memory. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
85 |
|
The term associative memory includes two accepted meanings -- (1) a learning process, (2) a recognition process, by which a memory is recalled on the basis of a fraction of it. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
85 |
|
Prominent among the network memory models are the self-organizing model of Edelman (1987). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
86 |
|
Enormous capacity of the brain for parallel processing, unlike
conventional computers. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
86 |
|
Two fundamental features of cortical processing and memory -- (1) probabilistic nature of cortical
processing, and (2) robustness of the memory. In cortical neurophysiology, variance is the rule; no certain output comes from any
input. The robustness
of memory is demonstrated by its resistance to brain injury; certain
cortical injuries may not affect memory at all or, if they do, they affect it
only temporarily. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
87 |
|
Growth of the Network |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
87 |
|
Perception
is an act of classification performed by nets of
interconnected cortical cells on the qualities of objects and events. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
88 |
|
The qualities of objects and
events upon which perception
makes a classification are a set of relations
previously established in a neural net by simultaneously
occurring experiences. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
88 |
|
All perception is categorical in that it is an interpretation of an object or event made in the
light of past experience by the network, which acts as a classifying
apparatus of cortical
connections. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
88 |
|
The system of connections for classifying stimuli is hierarchical. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
88 |
|
Hierarchy
of multiple
classification (I.e. classes
of classes of classes, etc.) will correspond to
the order of sensory qualities previously established by experience. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
88 |
|
The memory
network, or topological map, is highly dynamic. With experience, it will not only grow upward to form ever higher
hierarchies of classes but downward and sideways, to form new classes within previously existing ones, to reclassify objects, events, and their qualities. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
89 |
|
Categorical
and hierarchical character of all perception and memory. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
89 |
|
Hierarchical organization of connective systems in cortical areas of association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
89 |
|
Dynamic interaction between perception and memory,
which conforms to the almost complete overlap of processing and representational
networks in the cortex. [Edelman's dynamic core] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
90 |
|
Hayek's model successfully
avoids the problem of the so-called "grandmother
cell" because, in it, categorical
representation is dispersed in a wide network of interconnections. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
90 |
|
Edelman's theory of neuronal group selection starts by assuming a genetic
endowment of neuron groups, such as the columnar modules of the cortex,
with an inherent degree of variability and plasticity in their connections.
They constitute the units of selection of the primary repertoire. |
|
0 |
Fuster;
Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
90 |
|
The neural
network is an amalgam of many associative neuronal groups
selected by prior experience. When offered a new input signal, many subsets of neuronal connections within the network will
recognize (closely conform to) a signal similar to ones that previously have fashioned the
network (accumulated pattern of synaptic
strengths via Hebbian processes). Many neuronal
groups (synaptically-connected patterns) can respond more or less well to an
input signal. The tolerance for
variation in the signal (degeneracy), ensures wide distribution of
information and a degree of stimulus constancy -- i.e. equivalents of response to variants of the same stimulus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
91 |
|
The system conceived via
Edelman's theory of neuronal group selection is selective and adaptive, like evolution -- hence the name
of "neural Darwinism" which Edelman coined. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
91 |
|
Edelman's Neural
Darwinism shares with Hayek's model the merits of
distribution of information, probabilistic
response, and
robustness. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
91 |
|
Growth of the network in Edelman's model, as in Hayek's model, is unsupervised and self-organized. It does not follow
preestablished rules as in supervised learning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
It seems certain that the growth
of any neuronal network is
predicated on the presence of considerable plasticity, even in the adult organism. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
Plasticity
means the capacity not only to strengthen
pre-existing synapses but to form new ones. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
Plasticity
also probably means the capacity to develop new axon terminals and new dendrites and dendritic spines. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
It is function and usage that ultimately will determine the direction of connectivity and expansion of the network. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
Lack of function and usage in the network is bound to lead to
regression and competitive loss. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
"Use it or lose it" probably expresses the very
essence of what happens. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
It seems reasonable to postulate
that cortical memory networks
grow in the general direction of ontogeny -- i.e. away from primary areas and into the areas of association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
It seems logical that memory networks expand toward ever higher levels of the processing
hierarchies for perception and for action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
In memory
networks the connectivity
up the hierarchies include convergence as well as divergence every step of the way
and, and addition, there is recurrence and backflow. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
It is unclear precisely how much
of the cortical wiring is genetically or epigenetically layed out and how
much of it has developed during, and as the product of, individual life experiences (i.e.
by learning). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
94 |
|
If the presumption of network
expansion into association areas is correct, networks can be envisioned to grow from phyletic memory into individual
memory, as a general trend. Thus the variability
in connectivity and in function should increase in the direction of network growth. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
A group or
class of stimuli that have been associated into a network can, by subsequent encounters, become associated with other stimuli that did not originally belong to the network. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
For a new stimulus to be recognized, it is required only
that a member stimulus or feature of the original class co-occur with any given new stimulus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
In an associative
network in higher cortex, an entire class in
memory, rapidly can establish new connections with incoming stimuli. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
Stimuli of two different classes can, by
co-concurrent, link the two classes together to form a new supraordinate class, and this one, in turn, can
associate with others
at the same or lower levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
A memory
network can accrue associations in different levels of the representational hierarchy, up
and down. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
An abstract
concept at the top of
the hierarchy is based on the entire network or networks that
supported it in their connections and is widely
distributed. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
There will be some concentration of conceptual and semantic representation in certain areas of confluent associations, such as Wernicke's area in posterior association cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
Ultimately it is function and usage that create the system of multiple interconnected networks
for representation and classification of sensory information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
95 |
|
Intimate and mutual
relationships between experience and representation,
between perception and memory. Both use the very
same networks to categorize
information and to retain
it. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
96 |
|
Perception
and memory both use
the very same networks
to categorize information and to retain it. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Memory is a
cortical network, an array of connected links formed by
experience between neurons of the neocortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Memory networks of the lowest and most primitive
components of individual
memory can be viewed as
topological feature maps, in that their neurons
encode sensory and motor features. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
At higher
levels of individual memory, the concept of
feature becomes progressively more dependent on idiosyncratic connections and less
on concrete physical parameters. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Modular organization of primary
sensory cortex disappears
almost completely as we proceed into associational cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
At all levels and for all kinds
of networks, the information
that networks contain is defined by the structure
of each network -- i.e. by its neuronal elements and the connections that link them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Many different network share their constituent elements,
cells and connections. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Lowest level constituents of a perceptual network are the columnar modules of sensory cortex or some other suitable group of functionally homogeneous
neurons. They are the "feature
detectors," which encode primitive attributes of the sensory environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
On the motor side, the lowest
constituents of the network are the groups of cells in motor
cortex that, by their concerted action, determine
a vector of movement in a given part of the body, a motor
feature.
(FAPs) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
Perceptual and motor networks permeate upward
through the sensory and motor hierarchies and, as they
do, they become generally more complex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
As pathways
converge, networks also converge and share with others their more abstract and categorical aspects. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
High up in the hierarchies, the
nodes of networks are simply other, more discrete, networks that are
subcomponents of associative memory representing its relatively concrete
aspects. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
97 |
|
At higher levels of a hierarchy,
subordinate networks take the place that tightly interconnected clusters of
cells occupy at lower levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
Many networks overlap one another and share the same connections and
cells. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
Any connection at any neuron can be a member of many different
networks, of many
different memories. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
A distributed perceptual or
motor cortical network should be two-dimensional on the cortical plane. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
All associational connectivity
between areas is horizontal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
Functional differentiation is
greater on the horizontal than on the vertical plane. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
On the horizontal plane,
cortical connectivity displays the combinatorial power by which it can make
practically infinite networks and practically infinite memories. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
A conceptual network would be a
large network involving broad areas of unimodal and polymodal association
cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
98 |
|
Because a conceptual
network would include
parts of many networks with common connections, it would be not
only widely distributed
but also robust. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
99 |
|
Multiple access and wide
distribution are compatible with concentration of conceptual and semantic
representation in areas a convergent association. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
99 |
|
Recurrent feedback connections form a part of the processing
hierarchies of which they are a part. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
99 |
|
Reentry
allows the network to maintain reverberating activity within it, a feature of utmost importance for short-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
99 |
|
Access of the network to lower
levels can be important in acquisition of new memory, discrimination, and
selective attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
101 |
|
Widely separated groups of
neurons in primary visual (striate) cortex respond to a visual stimulus with
highly correlated trains of oscillations (30 -- 80 Hz, approximately 40 Hz as
a rule). |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
113 |
|
6 - Organization of perceptual
memory |
|
12 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
113 |
|
Individual memories are self-organized neuronal
networks of potentially infinite variety that
extend beyond primary sensory and motor cortex into the so-called cortex of
association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
113 |
|
In the recognition
and retrieval of a memory, mainly pyramidal cells are induced to fire at frequencies
above their "spontaneous" baseline. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
113 |
|
While the net is active, new memory can be added to it through the creation of new associations in accord with the principle of synchronous
convergence. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
113 |
|
It is in the active state that a
memory network controls behavior and presumably gains access to
consciousness. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
114 |
|
Visual Memory |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
128 |
|
Tactile Memory |
|
14 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
139 |
|
Auditory Memory |
|
11 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
139 |
|
Notorious and paradoxical
difficulty that such an intelligent animal as a rhesus monkey has for
learning fine auditory discriminations. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Input from both the ears is almost equally distributed in the primary acoustic cortex of
both sides. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Primary acoustic cortex -- Heschl's transverse gyrus in the upper temporal plane, most
of the hidden in the Sylvian fissure. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Memory for words and sentences
is inextricably tied to all human memories. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Language is
learned in the context of other experience and recalled, implicitly or explicitly, as part
of that context. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Words, and
the concepts for which they stand, are associated by experience with a vast variety of non-auditory material. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
The large sector of individual long-term memory that is
called semantic memory
is widely distributed
in the cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Long-term
semantic memory can
be accessed through a
multiplicity of
perceptual channels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
In the dominant
hemisphere, the representations of words and language appear idiosyncratically localized and tied to experience. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Semantic aphasia is difficult to differentiate from acoustic
agnosia. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
140 |
|
Auditory and language areas on
the left hemisphere. (diagram) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
141 |
|
Receptive or semantic aphasia is the agnosia for words and sentences. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
141 |
|
Acoustic agnosia is the perceptual amnesia of nonverbal auditory material,
such as music (amusia) and
sounds. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
141 |
|
Large conglomerate of cortical association areas of parietal, temporal, and occipital cortex that have come to be named,
PTO cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
141 |
|
PTO region of cortex includes unimodal
association cortex of three
modalities -- auditory,
visual, and tactile
-- as well as the polymodal association cortex of the supramarginal gyrus, the angular gyrus, and the
posterior third of the superior temporal gyrus (Wernike's
area). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
141 |
|
Musical sight reading and keyboard
performance have been noted to activate
metabolically, cortical areas that are adjacent to, though not identical with, Wernike's area and Broca's area. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
142 |
|
Research studies of auditory agnosias and aphasias, both of which reflect
disorders of long-term memory, as well as studies of auditory short-term
memory, provide indirect evidence that the cortical substrates for short and long-term memories are identical. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
142 |
|
Disconnection syndromes illustrate how the different
components of one memory network interact with one another. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
142 |
|
Disconnection syndromes provide
much suggestive evidence that one and the same engram can be accessed from multiple
memory sources, the same sources through which
presumably, the polymodal information was gathered that originally
shaped the network. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
142 |
|
Left temporal lesions, unlike
right temporal ones, cause auditory memory deficits if the material to be
retained is verbal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
142 |
|
Tonal memory is impaired by
right temporal but not left temporal lesions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
144 |
|
Interaction between modalities in memory networks and the apparent identity
of stores for short-term
and long-term memory. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
144 |
|
Canadian investigators Penfield and Perot (1963),
electrically stimulated the brain of conscious neurosurgical patients being
treated for epilepsy. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
144 |
|
Penfield and
Perot responses to electrical
stimulation in epilepsy
patients -- hallucinatory perceptions, fragments of poorly defined sensory experience, music from right side stimulation;
a few visual responses |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
149 |
|
Gustatory and Olfactory Memory |
|
5 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
156 |
|
Polymodal and high-level
perceptual memory |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
157 |
|
Hierarchical diagram (diagram) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
161 |
|
7 - Organization of Motor Memory |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
161 |
|
Motor memory is the neural
representation of motor acts and behavioral sequences. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
162 |
|
Neural Hierarchy of Motor Memory |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
165 |
|
Schematic diagram of the
circuitry of the motor system emphasizing the parallel organizations a
cerebellum and basal ganglia pathways.
(diagram) |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
167 |
|
Basal ganglia
in general, and the striatum in particular, may be viewed is harboring a stack of intermediate representational layers
of the motor hierarchy
that extends from the spinal cord to the cortex. [Stereotyped motor
programs] [FAPs] |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
167 |
|
Basal ganglia
and striatum nuclei in particular, at the base of the
cerebrum, represents some of the programs for the execution of automatized actions and habits. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
At the bottom of the motor
memory hierarchy, represented in the circuitry of the spinal cord, pons, and
cerebellum, lie the genetic representations of action, the inborn reflexes. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
The hypothalamus is the diencephalic base of instinctual programs representing
the biological motor memory of the species. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
In the basal
ganglia, we encounter programs and structures of acquired and temporally extended action that reach their culmination, in terms of complexity, in the
prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
The most automatic acts of all are the spinal reflexes. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
At higher levels, in the cerebellum and basal ganglia, learned automatic acts are
represented -- acts that, by replication and practice, have become habit. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
168 |
|
The dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex, at the top of the motor hierarchy, is
essential for planning of future voluntary action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
169 |
|
Motor programs |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
171 |
|
There is a running interaction between motor programs
at different hierarchical levels. Anatomical evidence
of cortical-subcortical loops of connectivity linking frontal
areas to basal ganglia and lateral thalamus. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
171 |
|
Topographically organized
representations of movement. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
172 |
|
Motor programs in primary areas seem to migrate
to lower levels after acquisition and practice. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
173 |
|
Cortical Motor Representation |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
173 |
|
Cortical motor representation |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
174 |
|
Prefrontal cortex |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
182 |
|
Premotor cortex |
|
8 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
187 |
|
Motor Cortex |
|
5 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
190 |
|
Speech representation |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
197 |
|
8 - Dynamics of cortical memory
-- retrieval and attention |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
198 |
|
Memory retrieval |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Memories
are retrieved by associative access through their component representations, by reconstruction from fragments. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Active retrieval depends on eliciting representations by stimuli, external or
internal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Retrieving stimuli can be enormously diverse. An effective stimulus
might be a word or command, a visual image, an odor, an instinctual urge, or even an internal
representation or thought, abstract or concrete. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
The association of an evocative stimulus with the memory it evokes may have been formed at any previous time. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Retrieval
is basically an associative process, whether a person is asked, or is prompted by circumstances, to elicit a memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Phenomenon of priming is also based on associative retrieval, even though
the association
between retrieving stimulus and the priming cue may be covert, unconscious, and part of
long-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Retrieval
of motor memory is
basically no different from that of perceptual
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
199 |
|
Motor memories are elicited by stimuli associated with motor actions they represent. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
201 |
|
All processes of recognition, recall, and remembering have one common feature
-- access to a memory
through a stimulus or set of stimuli that at one time or another has become,
co-concurrent, an associated element of the memory and, therefore, of the neural network. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
201 |
|
Retrieval Mechanisms |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
201 |
|
Recognition
is a form of memory retrieval. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
210 |
|
In everyday life, both serial and parallel processing of sensory information must be constantly ongoing in the brain on
a massive scale. |
|
9 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
210 |
|
Which stimulus or stimuli will
prevail and which memory or memories will be activated at any given time must
depend on innumerable imponderables of the situation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
210 |
|
Neither a stimulus nor the
memory it evokes need be conscious. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Covert recognition. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
State dependent learning --
learning and memory by co-concurrence or near co-concurrence of external and
internal stimuli. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Some people become sick at the mere sight or mention of the boat. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
In state-dependent conditioning,
as an aversion learning, the relationship between the retrieving stimulus and
the memory itself may be highly symbolic and need not be conscious. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Priming
involves retrieval
under unconscious influences. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Primed recognition is probably influenced or mediated by a hidden -- unconscious and symbolic -- association of the retrieving stimulus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Forgetting |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
211 |
|
Forgetting
is the inability to retrieve memory despite conscious effort to do so. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
A characteristic of short-term memory is its vulnerability to interference. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
All forms of interference are potentially deleterious for the same reason -- the limited
capacity of short-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
Limit to
what can be retained at any given time in short-term
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
Forgetting
of a long-term memory is more complex than short-term memory -- (1) Initially poor fixation of the memory because of
inattention all lack of rehearsal, (2) affective state and motivation at the
time of acquisition, (3) disinterest that accompanies even mild depression,
(4) scattered attention euphoria -- all may adversely determine how
effectively we later remember facts, events, faces, names. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
Infantile amnesia -- no one is capable of
remembering distinctly the experiences of early infancy, especially those
before the age of 30 months. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
212 |
|
One reason for infantile amnesia
is that the mechanisms of active memory have not developed. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
In the second year of life
children acquire the capacity to recall simple events and sequences of
actions. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
Motor memory (including habits)
seems to develop faster than perceptual memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
Perceptual memory lags behind
motor memory because perceptual memory relies on the associative power of
words, which is underdeveloped in the prelingual child. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
Memory and its retrieval are
based on association. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
Best strategy for retrieval of a
recalcitrant memory is to use alternative associations as lines of access to
its network. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
Normal aging is commonly
associated with some degree of memory loss. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
213 |
|
It is not clear whether the
forgetfulness of the elderly is mainly due to poor memory fixation, loss of
stored information, or loss of the capacity to recall. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
214 |
|
Hippocampus has been noted to
undergo involutional changes. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
214 |
|
Age-related loss of synapses by
entorhinal axons on dendritic spines of hippocampal cells. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
214 |
|
Functional disorders of the
hippocampus could be the basis of age related troubles of the human in memory
acquisition as well as memory recall. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
214 |
|
Normal aging would bring about a
gradual degradation of hippocampal structure and mechanisms. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Much of the impaired capacity to
recall that affects some of the aged may be attributed to the loss of stored
information -- I.e. to the deterioration of cortical memory networks. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Structure of the human neocortex
does not seem to age uniformly. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Cell shrinkage and death appear
to especially affect associative areas of the frontal and temporal lobes. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Involution of neurons with age
seem to affect most severely the dendrites and the synaptic contacts on them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Notably impaired in the human,
especially after age 50, are the basilar dendrites of pyramidal cells in the
supragranular layers of Wernike's area, in the superior temporal gyrus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
215 |
|
Given the importance of Wernike's area for semantic memory and of the
corticocortical connections with supragranular termination for the activation
of cortical networks, it is reasonable to infer that dendritic degeneration is at the
root of the often-observed age-related impairment of recall
of names. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
216 |
|
Semantic memory is more resistant to cortical damage than is declarative memory. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
216 |
|
Memory of facts,
word meanings, and ideas is determined by varied and multiple
associations. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
216 |
|
Semantic networks have many more lines of associative access (including verbal associations) than episodic or declarative networks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
217 |
|
Mechanisms of attention |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
217 |
|
Both sensory
and motor systems have the means for selecting information among alternatives. This is what we call selective attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
217 |
|
Little is known about attention's neural mechanisms. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
217 |
|
Motor attention or "set." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
Attention
is the perceptual categorization that, at a given time, the organism actively
selects to fulfill its adaptive needs. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
Set for
action (motor
attention) is the mirror image, on the output
side, of the categorization of the sensory world that is required on the
input side to make efficient use of limited system capacity. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
Selective attention and selective perception can be used interchangeably |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
Intensive aspect of attention we commonly call alertness. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
A certain minimum level of alertness is a precondition of attention and can be an enhancer of
all inputs regardless of their source |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
218 |
|
Substrate of alertness seems to lie in the primordial subcortical structures of the brainstem, from where it can modulate the entirety of the cerebral cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
219 |
|
Alertness |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
219 |
|
Reticular core of the upper brain stem has long been considered the essential neural substrate of alertness or vigilance. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
219 |
|
Electrical stimulation of the reticular
activating system awakens
animals from sleep. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
219 |
|
Reticular lesions induce coma and lethargy. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
219 |
|
Several neurotransmitter systems
(monoaminergic and cholinergic) with widespread cortical modulating
influences have nuclei of origin or pathways within the reticular formation
of the upper brain stem. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
222 |
|
Inputs from
limbic structures can
affect cortical activity and attention, although less massively and more selectively than brainstem inputs. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
222 |
|
Amygdala
most likely plays a role in recognizing the motivational
significance of any external
stimulus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
222 |
|
Amygdala can activate the
network retrogradely on arrival of the memory-retrieving stimulus, thus
facilitating its analysis and categorization. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
222 |
|
Connections
between amygdala and association cortex are numerous, reciprocal, and widespread. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
224 |
|
Selective attention |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
When the retrieved
memory is a temporally
extended structure of behavior (a behavioral Gestalt) with component sensory and motor
representations, we can trace the activity of the
network as it becomes operational and as behavior is implemented. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
As processing goes from one part of the network
to another and the focus
of attention shifts
with it, the network
must reach back into previous
stages for finer
analysis. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
Perception-action cycle probably make use of recurrent
collaterals and through them the feedback excitation has the effect
of enhancing discrimination at lower levels and sharpening
the focus of attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
A reverse
and complementary side to attentive focusing probably Is
based on the opposite of
excitation -- inhibition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
When we attend to something in particular, everything else seems to be suppressed. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
Proverbial selective
listening at a noisy
party. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
Filter theory
of attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
Because of the limited capacity of sensory processing systems, attention is, above all, a filtering process. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
225 |
|
Preattended
or early scrutiny of
what comes in -- a kind of fast, parallel, and unconscious processing that selectively can retrieve
material from unattended
messages if relevant cues are embedded in them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
226 |
|
Essence of the attention system is the rejection of what is not intended, not the enhancement of what is intended. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
226 |
|
Excitatory process of
categorization with the lateral inhibition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
226 |
|
Auditory habituation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
226 |
|
Crick's conceptualization of the neural
basis of selective attention. His searchlight idea of attention, inhibitory influences from the reticular
nucleus of the thalamus would modulate cortical synapses to sharpen the encoding of certain stimuli and stimulus configurations. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
Perceptual attention is based on
the selective activation of certain areas of cortex involved in
representation and categorization of behaviorlly relevant sensory features. |
|
6 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
Inhibition
of neuronal groups
that represent nonrelevant features. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
Classes or categories
of action much as
there are classes or categories of perception. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
Areas in front
of the central fissure constitute the cortical
level of neural hierarchy for representation of processing of motor action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
At the
lowest cortical level in motor cortex, the finest and most specific movements
are represented. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
The most
complex, global, and temporally extended
structures of behavior
are represented in prefrontal cortex, which
constitutes the highest level of the motor hierarchy. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
232 |
|
Processing of behavioral sequence structures commences when the networks that represent them cease to be
merely representational and become operational, i.e. when they channel information to lower levels for motor enactment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Processing
of behavioral structures generally continues downward through motor cortex, engaging successive subcortical
loops and the pyramidal
tract. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Lower stages
of the motor processing hierarchy feed back to prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Symmetrical cortical
organization for representation and processing of sensation, on the one hand, and for representation
and processing of action, on the other. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Perceptual categories are represented
and processed up their hierarchy, from the lowest and most concrete to the most general. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Action categories are represented and processed down their hierarchy, from the most general to the most concrete. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
In both perception and motor hierarchies, viewed from the center to the periphery, large classes include smaller classes, the smaller nested within the
larger, in perception as well is in action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
The word set comes closest to characterizing motor
attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
(READY, set, GO) -- The word set connotes the qualities of adaptation and preparation for movement. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
All classes of action have
corresponding neural structures and mechanisms for motor set. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Frontal areas are stacked from
top down (prefrontal, premotor, and motor) for progressively more concrete
and immediate set and action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Perceptual attention succeeds
the alerting or memory-retrieving sensory stimulus, whereas motor set
precedes the consequent motor action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Intimate functional relationship
between perceptual action and motor set |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Orienting movements of the eyes and head
are at the service of perceptual attention and
are guided by it. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Motor set is strictly dependent
on sensory cues and proprioceptive inputs. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Constant interplay of perceptual attention and motor set at all levels of the perception-action
cycle. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
233 |
|
Set-related activity of neurons before movement has been substantiated in motor
cortex, premotor cortex, and prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
234 |
|
Prefrontal neuron involvement in
motor attention (set). |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
235 |
|
In the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex, which is the highest stage for sensorimotor integration in the time
domain, a neural basis for the transfer of the focus of attention from
perception to movement. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
237 |
|
9 - Dynamics of Cortical Memory
-- ACTIVE MEMORY |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
237 |
|
Active memory
is an active cortical
network, it's neurons
active above certain baseline or spontaneous level of firing. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
237 |
|
Active memory
as a dynamic state of cortical representation comes
closer to explaining a wide range of neural
phenomena of mnemonic storage. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
237 |
|
No evidence
of separate cortical systems for long- and short-term memory, for memory in permanent storage and memory in transient storage. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
237 |
|
Established memory -- cortical
layout of what we could call passive or latent memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Working memory is an operational concept of active memory. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Working memory can be defined as an ad hoc
memorization of a discrete
item of information for a motor or cognitive act to be
performed in the short term. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Working memory has been construed as one kind of short-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Active memory,
which may not have a behavioral purpose, subsumes and transcends working
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Active memory
does not have a specific
cortical locus or system, although the frontal cortex is exceedingly important for it. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Delay task
are the best and most widely used test of working memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
All sequential behaviors that are nonroutine necessitate a mechanism of temporal
information storage to attain their goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
No creative speech is possible
without active memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Without active memory, logical
thinking cannot reach its conclusions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Active memory
is part and parcel of any behavioral or cognitive gestalt. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
238 |
|
Topography of Active Memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
243 |
|
Cortical Dynamics and Memory
Cells |
|
5 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
247 |
|
Activation of Perceptual Memory |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
254 |
|
Computational Models of Active
Memory |
|
7 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
262 |
|
Activation of Motor Memory |
|
8 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
262 |
|
Frontal Networks and Active
Storage |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
266 |
|
Cooperation of Posterior and
Frontal Networks |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
268 |
|
Posterior
and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices exerted modulating influences on
each other in active
memory. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
269 |
|
By way of
reentrant connections, activated frontal cortex reciprocates posterior cortical input with excitatory output to the same portion of the posterior cortex
from which the perceptual input originates. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
269 |
|
By feedforward and feedback, the entire perceptual-motor
network, which spans
portions of both cortices, is activated and maintained active, as is the memory
it represents. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
270 |
|
From Motor Representation to
Motor Set |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
270 |
|
The focus of representation
gradually shifts from the perceptual to the motor sector of the network. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
270 |
|
As they frontal network
representing the action reaches a certain level of threshold, it sets the
subjacent levels of the motor hierarchy for execution of the action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
274 |
|
PERCEPTION -- ACTION CYCLE |
|
4 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
280 |
|
Edelman's diagram of the connectivities that subserve the
highest linguistic functions. |
|
6 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
283 |
|
10 - Phenomenology of Memory |
|
3 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
283 |
|
Conscious experience is the
essence of remembering, attention, and active memory, the three main aspects
of memory dynamics. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
284 |
|
SLEEP AND DREAMS |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
285 |
|
REM sleep is
important for memory consolidation. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
285 |
|
Humans deprived of REM sleep have difficulties in acquiring and retaining new information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
285 |
|
Learning new tasks seem to promote REM sleep. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
286 |
|
Acquisition of new learning depends on some kind of tonic
input to the cortex from subcortical nuclei that are active and can be further activated during REM sleep. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
286 |
|
Oscillatory 40-Hz bioelectrical
activity on the scalp of
humans in dream state. (Llamás, 1993) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
286 |
|
Oscillatory activity of various frequencies may be the expression of the recurrent
activity between cortex and thalamus that has been postulated
to be essential for the regulation of all sleep stages. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
286 |
|
Role of REM sleep in learning
needs further study. Learning related
to REM sleep enhancements seen generally weak, inconsistent and, to a large
extent, dependent on the task being learned. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
287 |
|
Dreaming is consistent with the
activation of cortical memory networks. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
288 |
|
CONSCIOUSNESS AND MEMORY
DYNAMICS |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
288 |
|
In the waking
state, the ensemble of nonconscious
cognitive structures and processes is constantly at work. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
289 |
|
Both memory
retrieval and attention rely on fast parallel processing that
occurs at an unconscious level. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
290 |
|
Unconscious knowledge, which has been termed "implicit
memory." |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
290 |
|
Implicit memory and its apparent
subcomponents, as well as priming. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Perceptual memory and motor memory, represented in postrolandic and prerolandic (frontal) cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Central sulcus is a prominent landmark of the
brain, separating the parietal
lobe from the frontal
lobe and the primary
motor cortex from the primary
somatosensory cortex. Central
sulcus was originally called the fissure of
Rolando or the Rolandic fissure, after Luigi Rolando. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Much of motor memory, including
habits and conditioned reflexes, is represented in basal ganglia, cerebellum,
and other subcortical structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Lesions of posterior cortex are
likely to result in perceptual amnesias, whereas frontal lesions are likely
to result in motor amnesias. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Semantic memory is anchored in
larger networks with more associations than declarative memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Small posterior lesions may
affect only the episodic or declarative aspects of perceptual memory, whereas
larger posterior lesions may, in addition, affect the semantic and more
abstract memories and knowledge. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Under normal circumstances, it
is easier to retrieve memories based on many associations (i.e. large cortical networks with many connections) than memories
based on only a few. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
A well known and often effective
maneuver to reach recondite
memory is
successively to test in our mind several
associative links to it. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
When our sensory
apparatus detects something
out of the ordinary, something that does not
quite match memory or meet expectations, then attention is mobilized and focused on that something. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
From preconscious
preattention, our mind shifts to conscious attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
Within the focus of selective
attention, the processing is deployed for the analysis, discrimination, and
categorization of the world. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
291 |
|
In the retrieval
and activation of memory that go on in ordinary behavior, cortical
processing can be conceived to take place along many channels of an extensive network. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
Consciousness
is commonly manifested by
high-frequency (~40 Hz) oscillatory activity in certain brain structures. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
High-frequency (~40 Hz)
oscillatory activity has been supposed to reflect a correlative neural
function important for perceptual binding and attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
In the transition from
perception to action (Perception-Action Cycle), the spread of excitation
takes place presumably and chiefly from posterior to frontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
Attention
probably is accomplished by a degree of feedback excitation on sensory stages. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
In active memory, the focus of
attention has been internalized, no longer directed to external objects but
to internal representations. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
293 |
|
Most active
memory consists of reactivated
long-term memory; it is experienced as an
expansion of the present, the "remembered
present" (Edelman) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Active short-term memory can be considered the
quintessential conscious phenomenon. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Neither consciousness nor active
memory has a definite locus in the brain. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
VOLITION |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Act of will is conscious. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Willful motor action is preceded
and accompanied by unconscious processing. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Parallelism of perceptual and
motor attention, although the two affect vastly different spheres of
processing. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Perceptual attention leads to discrimination and
analysis of the sensory
world. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Motor attention leads to intention and to the willful motor act. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
If perceptual
attention is the selective
categorization of sensory
information, motor
attention or set is the selective categorization of motor action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Both perceptual attention and
motor attention are, to a large degree, determined by prior experience (I.e.
by perceptual and motor memory) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
In motor
attention, in the preparation
for the motor act and concomitant with it, much parallel, fast, and unconscious processing takes place. Similarly, in perceptual
attention and in retrieval
of perceptual memory, much parallel, fast, and unconscious processing takes place. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Much of our motility, even in
the midst of the most conscious and deliberate behavior, takes place
automatically and unconsciously. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Much of our motility is based
thoroughly on experience (i.e. on long-term motor memory). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
As any pianist,
typist, or skilled
worker will testify to, consciousness can be an impediment to performance. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Much motor
performance is preorganized and occurs at a preattentive and preconscious level. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
294 |
|
Selective motor attention takes place consciously, and at least partly in series, setting
the motor apparatus for deliberate, usually novel or complex action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Bulk of automatic
motility is likely is represented, organize, and
executed in subcortical structures, including parts of the diencephalon,
basal ganglia, and cerebellum. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Parts of the diencephalon,
basal ganglia, and cerebellum is where most of the fast and
unconscious processing of motor set takes place. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Neurophysiological evidence to
indicate that before a willful act, some of the automatic motility processing
takes place in the cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Voluntary act
is preceded by some unconscious processing somewhere
and the frontal lobe cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Frontal activation, of which the subject is unaware, seems to play a role in preattentive
motor set. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Some researchers have estimated
that readiness potential
originates in the supplementary motor area (SMA). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Readiness potential may be part
of a larger frontal wave of surface negativity that originates in prefrontal
cortex before willful action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is one of the most richly
connected regions of the cerebrum. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
In addition to
receiving afferents from subcortical structures involved in motor control, prefrontal and premotor cortices receive abundant afferents from limbic and brainstem regions that are clearly implicated in drive and motivation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Prefrontal and premotor cortices also receive profuse connections from a
large array of areas of posterior neocortex that are implicated in perception and perceptual memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
295 |
|
Perceptual cortical networks of
posterior cortex expanded into the frontal lobe and blend there with motor
networks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
296 |
|
It is through connections to the
perceptual cortex networks of posterior cortex that frontal neuron assemblies
are recruited for active memory and for the organization of motor action. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
296 |
|
Perception-action cycle places frontal cortices under the influence of the posterior
cortices and subcortical
structures, within a circular
array in which there
is no true origin. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
296 |
|
Voluntary action is probabilistically determined by resolution of conflict, in frontal
cortex, between competing neural assemblies of diverse origin. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
296 |
|
Free will is as free as the
probabilistically-determined influences are unconscious and as other
seemingly plausible options are available. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
296 |
|
Some of the unconscious
influence may come from identified cortical areas
that represent our higher values -- from a fund of abstract perceptual and motor memory built by education and good example of fellow humans. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Memory in Cerebral Cortex |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|