Joaquín
Fuster; The Prefrontal Cortex |
|
|
Book |
Page |
|
Topic |
|
|
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
1 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is defined as the part of the
cerebral cortex that receives projections from
the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. |
|
|
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
2 |
|
Entirety of the frontal cortex, including his
prefrontal region, is "action cortex." It is cortex
devoted to action of one kind or another. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
2 |
|
Internal, mental, action that we call reasoning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
2 |
|
Frontal cortex is a "doer
cortex," much as the posterior cortex is "sensor cortex." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
2 |
|
Frontal cortex does nothing by itself; all it does is in cooperation with other cortices, with subcortical structures, and with certain sectors of the sensory and motor apparatus of the
autonomic nervous system. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
2 |
|
There is considerable specialization of action within the frontal cortex (action domains). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
As organisms
evolve, their actions
become more complex and more idiosyncratic, their goals more remote in space and time, and their reasons or motives for attaining
them more covert, less transparent, more based on prior experience than on presumptive instinctual need. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
As organisms evolve, the lateral or outer frontal convexity, which is essential for cognitive functions and intelligent behavior, undergoes greater development than that of the medial and inferior (orbital) surfaces, which is critically involved in emotional behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
Only the lateral
prefrontal cortex can provide the "temporal gestalt" with
the coherence and coordination of actions that are essential for the organism to reach its goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
Capacity of the prefrontal cortex to organize actions in the time domain, which is the most general and characteristic
of all prefrontal
functions in the primate. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
Temporal organizing function in mammalian behavior provides higher
reasoning and creative activity rather than the minimal
temporal dimension of the here and now,
instinctual sequence or automatic
routine. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
All cortical functions take place on a neural substrate of modifiable, long-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
The representational substrate
of the prefrontal cortex, in
particular its lateral
sector,
is made of networks of executive memory, which extend into other cortical areas and have been formed by prior
experience. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
3 |
|
Executive functions or operations of the prefrontal
cortex essentially consist of the utilization of
that substrate (1) for the acquisition of further executive memory and (2)
for the organization of behavior, reasoning, and language. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Planning
and decision making
are two of the major executive functions of the prefrontal
cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Executive attention has three critical components, all three direct participants
in the goal-directed temporal organization of action: (1) working memory, (2)
preparatory set, and (3) inhibitory interference control. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Prefrontal cortex performs its executive control of temporal organization by orchestrating activity in other
neural substrates that participate in executive attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Unitary view of memory with a common cortical substrate -- working memory is a temporary
activation of updated long-term memory networks for orchestrating actions in the
near term. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Working memory is essentially sustained
attention focused on an internal
representation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
4 |
|
Selective
activation of posterior cortical areas by the prefrontal cortex, in that process of internal attention that we call working memory, it is a major
aspect of the neural basis of what has been called "cognitive
control." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
5 |
|
Since 1971, when prefrontal "memory
cells" were first recognized, working
memory, by whatever name, has become the cardinal executive function of the prefrontal cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster;
Prefrontal Cortex |
5 |
|
Notwithstanding the conceptual "Balkanization" of the prefrontal cortex into a topographic quilt of areas dedicated to a succession of supposedly independent cognitive
or emotional functions, (1) all prefrontal functions and areas are to some degree interdependent, and (2) the various functions share areas and networks in common. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
Perception-action cycle consists of coordinated
participation of neural structures in the successive interactions
of
the organism with its environment in pursuit of goals. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
Perception-action cycle is a basic
biological principle of cybernetic processing between the organism and its environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
Prefrontal cortex constitutes the highest stage of neural integration in the perception-action
cycle. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
In the course of a goal-directed sequence of actions, signals from internal milieu and the external environment are processed through hierarchically
organized neural channels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
At each
hierarchical level of
the perception-action cycle, there is feedback to prior levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
At the highest
level of the perception-action
cycle,
there is reentrant
feedback from the prefrontal
cortex to the posterior
association cortex, which plays a critical role in working memory,
set, and monitoring. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
6 |
|
Role of the prefrontal
cortex in coordinating
cognitive functions, including the neural
structures involved, in the temporal organization of behavior
-- i.e. the formation of coherent behavioral sequences
toward the attainment of goals. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
7 |
|
Anatomy of
the prefrontal cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
13 |
|
Development
of neuronal architecture
in human prefrontal cortex. (diagram) |
|
6 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
15 |
|
Immaturity
of the prefrontal cortex at birth. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
15 |
|
Last to myelinate are the association
areas. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
15 |
|
Later myelinating areas engage in complex functions highly related
to the experience of
the organism. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
17 |
|
Development
of frontal, especially
prefrontal, cortex does not reach its completion before the third decade of life or later. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
17 |
|
Higher cognitive functions for which the prefrontal cortex is essential -- language, intelligence, and reasoning, which rely heavily on the front cortical and cortico-cortical connectivity
-- do not reach full maturity until the third decade of life. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
Two weeks before birth, cortico-cortical and cortico-caudate projections of the cortex have
already reached their adult
targets and distributions. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
Newborns
essentially possess a large fraction of the connective apparatus that the prefrontal cortex will need to interact with other cortical areas and with its principal outlet
structures for motor
control. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
At every stage of ontogenetic development, the structural
phenotype of the cortex is not only subject to genetic
factors but to a variety of internal and external influences. Critical among these influences are those
that derive from the interactions of the organism with its environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
Executive networks grow in the prefrontal cortex and are
essentially selective; they select neurons and circuits among those
that have been overproduced in earliest stages of development, while other neurons and terminals undergo
regression and disappearance. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
"Selective
stabilization" is the result of competition for inputs on the part of neurons and terminals. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
18 |
|
Age-related
cell regeneration and loss. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
19 |
|
In the seventh
or eighth decade of the life of the human,
several manifestations of involution are consistently found in the prefrontal
cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
19 |
|
Much of the decrement in neuronal size is attributable to shrinkage and
disappearance of dendrites. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
20 |
|
Cytoarchitectural differences between cortical areas may not be as important as are the patterns
of distribution of afferent fibers, internal
connectivity,
and the destination of efferent fibers. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
21 |
|
Brodmann (1909) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
21 |
|
Origin of the
"prefrontal" designation is uncertain. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
21 |
|
A more sensible criterion for definition of the prefrontal cortex is the distribution of thalamic fibers. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
21 |
|
Hodologically defined prefrontal cortex -- i.e. of the cortical projection area of the medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
26 |
|
Brodmann's areas 44 and 45 is the seat of Broca's speech
area. |
|
5 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
27 |
|
Axon terminals and collaterals of some prefrontal pyramidal
neurons extends several millimeters horizontally. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
27 |
|
Lateral connectivity suitable for intra-prefrontal
association and other interactions including lateral inhibition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
28 |
|
Mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus has two major
cytoarchitectonically different components: a medial component, termed "magnocellular" for the large size of its cells, and a lateral one with mostly small cells, termed "parvocellular." |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
28 |
|
Magnocellular portion of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus projects mainly to the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
28 |
|
Parvocellular portion of the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus projects mainly to the (newer)
cortex of the lateral
prefrontal convexity. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
28 |
|
Brodmann's area 8, the frontal
eye field. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
30 |
|
Prefrontal cortex receives direct afferents from the brainstem tegmentum, the pons, the
hypothalamus,
and the amygdala. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
31 |
|
Lateral prefrontal cortex, especially area 46, receives connections from the cerebellum. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
31 |
|
Other cerebellar-prefrontal
loops course through the basal ganglia and the lateral thalamus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
31 |
|
Temporal organization of motor actions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
33 |
|
Major bundles of fiber
projections from sensory association areas of posterior
cortex to frontal
cortex.
(diagram) |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
41 |
|
Connectivity of the prefrontal cortex with structures
involved in motor function. (diagram) |
|
8 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
43 |
|
Connectivity of the prefrontal cortex with structures
involved in emotion. (diagram) |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
59 |
|
Chemical neurotransmission |
|
16 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
65 |
|
Six best-known neurotransmitters -- glutamate, GABA,
norepinephrine,
dopamine, serotonin, and acetylcholine. |
|
6 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
65 |
|
The most pervasive neurotransmitter, the amino acid GABA, is the prime inhibitory neurotransmitter in the central
nervous system, as prevalent in the prefrontal cortex as it is elsewhere. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
65 |
|
GABA is
largely a local
transmitter,
which serves interneurons and acts for the most part upon neighboring cells. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
65 |
|
The four neurotransmitters (norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, acetylcholine) originate in nuclear formations of the brain stem and project to orbitomedial and lateral prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
66 |
|
Glutamate |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
68 |
|
GABA |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
70 |
|
Norepinephrine |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
75 |
|
Dopamine |
|
5 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
85 |
|
Serotonin |
|
10 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
87 |
|
Acetylcholine |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
90 |
|
Neuropeptides |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
94 |
|
Schizophrenia |
|
4 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
98 |
|
Drug Abuse |
|
4 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
99 |
|
Critical connectivity in drug
addiction. (diagram) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
101 |
|
Depression |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
125 |
|
Animal neuropsychology |
|
24 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
133 |
|
Attention
is a neural biological function based on the limited capacity of neural processing systems as well
as the need to allocate that capacity to the most effective processing of information at any given time and in any given context. |
|
8 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
134 |
|
Attention
is a selective processing of the most relevant information. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
134 |
|
Attention
consists of two complementary cognitive processes -- (1) focusing and (2) inhibition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
134 |
|
Attention
is inseparable from other cognitive functions
such as sensory
perception and memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
134 |
|
Prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role in attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
138 |
|
Delay Tasks -- Working Memory |
|
4 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
138 |
|
Working memory -- temporary retention of information, sensory or other, for
the performance of a prospective act to solve a problem or to attain a goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
171 |
|
Human neuropsychology |
|
33 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Cortical function, in general, is
determined and defined by functional architecture -- i.e. by inputs, outputs, and intrinsic connective substrate. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
In primary sensory and motor areas, functional architecture is to some extent reducible to columns and modules with their discrete inputs and outputs. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
In the cortex
of association, including the
prefrontal cortex, the architecture is made of large, overlapping, distributed, and associative networks with multiple inputs and outputs of distant origin or destination. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Most of the cortex
of association networks are "individualized,"
made by personal experience in accord with Hebbian principles. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Prefrontal cortex as a whole is cortex dedicated to action in the widest sense of the word -- behavioral,
skeletal, ocular, vegetative, and cognitive action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Prefrontal cortex in its entirety is executive cortex, but its parts can
rarely be assigned any particular executive
function -- with the possible exception of area 8, for eye movements and executive visual attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Lateral prefrontal cortex is predominantly, but not
exclusively, involved in time integrating and organization functions, such as working memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
173 |
|
Medial and ventral prefrontal cortices are
predominantly involved in such emotional and social functions as the control of impulses, mood, and empathy. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
174 |
|
Apathy
usually accompanies the same cluster of symptoms associated with disorders of attention and general
motility. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
174 |
|
Apathy is antithetical not only to depression but also to anxiety. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
174 |
|
Depression |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
175 |
|
Caution before assuming that depression is a primary mood disorder and not secondary to disorders of cognitive function. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
175 |
|
Not uncommonly, patients with cortical pathology develop depression secondarily. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
175 |
|
Euphoria |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
175 |
|
Lesions of
the prefrontal cortex, even if they do not encroach on
premotor or motor cortex, can induced disorders of general motility. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
176 |
|
Hypokinesia
is characterized by a general dimunition of spontaneous motor activity. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
Theory of Mind
-- Empathy |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
"Theory of mind" is the ability of an individual to infer the feelings, motives, opinions, and emotions of another on the basis of his or her expressions, however fragmentary
or incomplete these may be. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
Theory of mind (ToM) is an indispensable ability for meaningful social interaction. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
Theory of mind is closely related to empathy, which ToM includes. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
Empathy
refers specifically to affective understanding ("emotional resonance"). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
177 |
|
ToM extends beyond affect to
include cognition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
180 |
|
Set is the preparation of neural resources for
expected sensory input
or motor response in the course of executive performance. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
180 |
|
The kind of
motor response to be prepared
for
depends on perception, recent memory, and long-term
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
180 |
|
Preparation of the the sensory
systems (perceptual set) or
motor system (motor set)
is geared to make the performance most efficient in pursuit of its goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
180 |
|
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
(WCST) has become the staple of formal neuropsychological testing of the frontal patient. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
182 |
|
Bistable figures test. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
182 |
|
Spatial neglect as a specific form of attention
deficit encountered in some prefrontal-injury patients. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
All executive
functions operate with a system of cognitive networks (cognits) widely distributed throughout the cortex. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Executive cognitive networks are made of associations between neuronal assemblies (gestalts), in some cases widely dispersed from one
another, which represents simpler
and more concrete items of knowledge and long-term
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Inasmuch as cognitive
networks contain association with action, they are executive networks and extend into the cortex of
the frontal lobe. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
High-level executive cognits, i.e. those that
represent goal-directed sequences of actions, especially if they are novel or prospective (plans),
extended into the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is the
depository of executive memory networks -- i.e. networks
that represent past actions, future actions, or both. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Executive memory network, constituting the neural substrate on which executive function will take
place, will be used in attention, working
memory, planning, etc.. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
At a given time, the executive memory network will cease to be only representational and will become
also operational to serve any or all of the executive functions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Frontal patients are ordinarily capable of forming
and retrieving perceptual long-term memory -- i.e. memory acquired through the senses. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Frontal patients usually have no difficulty with declarative or episodic memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Although frontal
patients are not
markedly amnesic, they have difficulties with both free recall and recognition. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
184 |
|
Frontal patients have disorders of executive
functions and temporal
integration. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Lesions of the orbito-limbic region of the prefrontal cortex can lead to spontaneous confabulation and false recall or recognition. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Interrelatedness of executive
function. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Close relationship between prefrontal executive functions and
between memory and planning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
A plan of action is made of associated elements of long-term executive memory bound together into a prefrontal
network that contains associations with future time and order. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Working memory is the ability
to retain an item of information for the prospective execution of an action that is dependent on
that information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Working memory is an essential cognitive
function for the mediation of cross-temporal contingencies in the temporal integration of reasoning, speech,
and goal
directed behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Working memory can fail in many pathological
conditions of the brain. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Working memory is necessary for prospective action, whether the action is a motor act, a mental
operation,
or a piece of spoken language. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Language
can be easily used by humans to categorize and to retain
information, and thus may serve as a source of mnemonic devices. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
185 |
|
Spatial information can be readily coded with language. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
186 |
|
Working memory can be characterized as sustained
attention to an internal
representation. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
189 |
|
Frontal patient's difficulty in temporally integrating behavior is only evident in challenging
situations and may not
surface in everyday life. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
190 |
|
We lead our daily
life through myriad
minor decisions that are determined by habit and the expectation of immediate fulfillment. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
190 |
|
Occasionally,
the opportunity and expectancy of higher reward -- financial gain, social approval or satisfaction of a
biological urge -- lead us to need to weigh
consequential choices in the face of uncertain outcome. Our decision is then determined by a number
of factors, most prominently the amount and timing
of potential reward and the degree of potential risk. [Bayesian inference] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
Because of the indeterminacy of rewards and risks,
the decision is then the result of probabilistic
estimates of both reward
and risk. [Bayesian
inference] |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
The estimates of both reward
and risk may be unconscious, in which case the
choice may be called an intuitive, based on so-called "gut
feeling." [Bayesian
inference] The
decision is emotionally biased. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
In reality, there is no purely
rational or purely emotional decision, as both reason
and emotion play a role in all decisions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
Prefrontal cortex takes part in all decisions, although its lateral regions are mainly involved in rational
factors, whereas its medial
and orbital regions of mainly involved in emotional factors. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
Lateral prefrontal cortex plays a major role in all decisions that are the result of temporal integration, working
memory, and planning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
Orbitomedial prefrontal cortex plays a major role in all decisions that are emotionally determined or biased. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
191 |
|
A patient with a ventromedial lesion is incapable of
making sound decisions because of insensitivity to future outcomes and
willingness to take unwise risks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Emotional contribution of orbitomedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Laterality
of orbitomedial function
-- more right side than left side involvement in social/emotional
functioning and decision-making. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Laterality may be less apparent
in women than in men. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Decision-making is the result of the cooperation
of all prefrontal areas. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Lateral prefrontal cortex
contributes to decision-making the veridical, multi-source,
"attribute-based" information within the temporal
gestalts of executive sequence. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
192 |
|
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex contributes to decision-making the
biologically adaptive,
"alternative based," value-guided information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
193 |
|
Best-known language disorder
resulting from frontal damage is aphasia from injury to the transitional cortex of the left inferior frontal gyrus, the Broca area (Brodmann's area 44 and
45). |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
194 |
|
Language and speech disorder (aphasia) - (diagram) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
194 |
|
Speech disorder areas of the
cortex -- lateral and medial views. (diagram) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
194 |
|
Lesions of the premotor cortex including the supplementary motor area
(SMA) in the medial
surface can cause efferent
aphasias somewhat similar to the Broca's,
although less marked. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
194 |
|
All language
disorders resulting from damage in the dominant hemisphere are more
severe than those from damage in the non-dominant
hemisphere. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
195 |
|
Intelligence
may be defined as the ability to adjust by
reasoning to new
changes, to solve new
problems, and to create value in new forms of action and expression. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
Intelligence
includes a number of components -- attention, reasoning, problem
solving, verbal expression, memory, abstraction,
and the ability to formulate behavioral plans and pursue them to their goal. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
All of the executive
capabilities are needed for the intelligent temporal integration of behavior, most certainly when the behavior is complex, original, and creative. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
For tests of
frontal lobe damage, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is more sensitive than the Stanford-Binet
Test. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
Creative intelligence is the capacity to create new goals, projects and plans. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
Contribution of the right hemisphere in general, and
the right frontal cortex
in particular, to creative intelligence. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
196 |
|
Large diversity of connections to prefrontal areas. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
197 |
|
Three major regions of the prefrontal cortex as defined by gross anatomy -- lateral, orbital, and medial. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
197 |
|
Lateral prefrontal cortex is the prefrontal cortex of the lateral convexity of the
frontal lobe, comprising parts or the entirety of areas 8, 9,10, and 46. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
197 |
|
Lateral, orbital, and medial
views of the cortex to show prefrontal Brodmann regions. (Diagram) |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
198 |
|
Orbitofrontal cortex is the cortex of the ventral
aspect of the frontal
lobe. It
comprises mainly areas 11 and 13. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Orbitofrontal patients may show by their behavior a blatant disregard for even the
most elementary ethical
principles. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Orbitofrontal syndrome is oftentimes indistinguishable from mania. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Criminal psychopathy is another psychiatric condition analogous in some respects to orbitofrontal syndrome. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Another clinical parallel is the
one between orbital syndrome and attention deficit
disorder (ADHD) of a hyperactive
child. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Medial prefrontal cortex comprises parts of areas 8 through
10, and areas 12, 24,
and 32. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Most of the medial
frontal cortex is involved in attention and somatic motility. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Lesions of the medial aspects of
areas 6 (SMA) and 8 frequently lead to difficulties in the initiation and
performance of limbic, eye, speech movements. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Lesions of the
anterior cingulate region generally lead to hypokinesia or akinesia. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
199 |
|
Area 24 is
at the crossroads of pathways linking the limbic
system with the frontal
lobe. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
In addition to testing working
memory, delay tasks test the capacity to suppress internal interference. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Child reaches the mature level of performance of executive function by age 10 to 12 years, relatively independent of IQ. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Planning
and executive memory
appear to have a similar timetable, possibly with rapid
development between the ages
of 6 and 9 years. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Formation and retention of motor patterns by 6-week-old infants. Imitate the actions of adults. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Eighteen-month-old children can not only reenact complex
series that they have witnessed
adults perform, they can also anticipate and create the serial acts needed to fulfill the adults intentions, as
the children intuit them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Well into adulthood, attention span increases, plans become more elaborate, goals include increasing
numbers of subgoals, decision-making is more dependent on deliberation, and
the capacity to use both inductive and deductive reasoning increases. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Prefrontal structural maturation progresses into the third decade of life. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Significant correlation between IQ and the increase in cortical thickness, notably in prefrontal regions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Temporal-integrative functions of the prefrontal cortex -- attention, memory, planning -- develop along with its structural
maturation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
202 |
|
Attention, memory, planning functions of the prefrontal cortex develop gradually, with spurts between 5 and 10 years of age, to reach completion about age 12. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
203 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is one of the last neocortical
regions to develop
structurally. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
203 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is one of the first to deteriorate with senescence. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
203 |
|
Generally, normal
aging is accompanied by a gradual diminution of attentive functions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
203 |
|
Onset of decline varies considerably between persons, usually appearing in the seventh or eighth decade of life. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
204 |
|
Influence of the environment in maintaining drive and attention. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
204 |
|
In the elderly, long-term memory and its retrieval are less affected than short-term memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
204 |
|
Age-dependent decline in executive functions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
221 |
|
Neurophysiology |
|
17 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
238 |
|
Organization and execution of movement down the executive hierarchy of the frontal lobe. |
|
17 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
238 |
|
Preparation for movement proceeds down the hierarchy from prefrontal to motor cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
238 |
|
Inhibitory control of motor
set in basal ganglia by orbital
prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
239 |
|
Posterior cortex and subcortical
structures are in
charge of the preparation (set) of the motor acts that constitute the behavioral sequence. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
239 |
|
Role of prefrontal
regions in attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
239 |
|
Working Memory |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
239 |
|
Adaptation of the micro-electrode recording techniques
to the behaving animal allowed
the exploration of cellular discharge in the prefrontal cortex of animals performing tasks for
which this cortex is deemed essential, notably delay
tasks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
243 |
|
"Memory cells," attributed to a short-term memory
function, which eventually we identified as working memory. |
|
4 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
243 |
|
Memory cells
can be found practically anywhere in prefrontal cortex, but most common in the area in and around the sulcus principalis.. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
244 |
|
Memory cells,
first found in the prefrontal cortex, were later found in other
cortical regions, as well as in the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
244 |
|
Other structures probably cooperate very closely with the prefrontal cortex and working memory, and their memory cells reflect that cooperation. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
244 |
|
Prefrontal memory cells are not so closely tuned to the physical properties of the
memorandum as some cells in sensory association areas of the inferotemporal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
252 |
|
Functional architecture of working memory models almost invariably includes reentry, but some of them include stabilizing
role of certain neurotransmitters, notably dopamine and GABA. |
|
8 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
252 |
|
GABA interneurons are responsible for the inhibition of distracting or irrelevant
information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
252 |
|
Reentrant circuitry with slow-acting NMDA receptors for persistent working memory activity. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
252 |
|
GABAergic interneurons for balance between excitation and inhibition, and for
inhibition of interference. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
254 |
|
Multiple feedback reentry may enable the storage of multiple
items in working
memory. |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
254 |
|
Dynamics of working
memory engages the
functional interactions of the prefrontal cortex with other structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
254 |
|
Reciprocal connections between the prefrontal cortex and the mediodorsal
nucleus of the thalamus. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
254 |
|
Reverberating circuitry for working memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
254 |
|
Most cortico-cortical
fibers originate and terminate in upper cortical layers. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
255 |
|
Functional interactions between prefrontal cortex and other cortices. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
255 |
|
Widely distributed nature of the cortical
substrate of working
memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
255 |
|
Controlling role of the prefrontal cortex over the
selection and maintenance of working memory content. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
285 |
|
Neuroimaging |
|
30 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
333 |
|
Overview of prefrontal
functions: Temporal organization of action |
|
48 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
334 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is "action cortex," "doer cortex," like the rest of the cortex of the
frontal lobe. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex in the parallel and circular paths of the perception-action cycle. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex works as an integrator of current inputs from cortical
and subcortical sources. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex operates under continuous
constraints from experience
(long-term memory), from the neural substrate of instinct and emotions, from current sensory input and motor output, and from feedback of both receptors and effectors. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Organism can produce no
sustained goal-directed action, except the most automatic
and routine, without the temporal integration functions of
the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex functions include working memory and the other functions under the heading of executive attention, which has
also been termed "cognitive control." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex includes cognitive infrastructure of goal-directed action. That
infrastructure is made of distributed,
overlapping, and
interactive neuronal networks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Prefrontal cortex infrastructure for goal-directed action
-- at low levels
of their hierarchical organization these networks are innate (phyletic memory); at higher levels they are formed by associative synaptic modulation
through life experience. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
All cognitive functions of the prefrontal cortex are dependent on
close interactions
with other cortices,
as well as several subcortical structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Widely distributed and interactive character of cortical cognitive
networks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
Cortical cognitive networks neurobiology, their distribution, their hierarchical organization, and the associative character of the memory they contain. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
All five essential
cognitive functions of the human brain -- attention, perception, memory, intelligence, and language -- consist of neural transactions between and within the cognitive networks of the cortex, termed "cognits." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
335 |
|
"Cognits" are cognitive
network units of knowledge and memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
336 |
|
Hierarchical organization of cognitive networks. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
336 |
|
Perceptual networks and
posterior cortex |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
339 |
|
Hierarchical organization of
memory networks (cognits) - (diagram) |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
340 |
|
Executive networks and frontal
cortex |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
342 |
|
Frontal action domains |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
344 |
|
Not all the constructs of action
are represented in the prefrontal cortex -- not the stereotyped instinctual
routines, or the sequences of automatic and well rehearsed acts (FAPs). |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
344 |
|
Procedural memory is stored in other structures, not
in prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
344 |
|
Prefrontal cortex intervenes in the representation and performance of a sequential task only during the initial stages of learning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
344 |
|
Engram of a
stereotyped task, the procedural memory of it, is migrated elsewhere from the
prefrontal cortex, possibly to hierarchically lower structures (e.g. premotor
cortex, basal ganglia). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
344 |
|
Prefrontal cortex has represented in it the relatively
novel variants of old
structures of action, in whatever domain. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
A structure
of action is a temporal
gestalt, like a melody. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Temporal gestalts obey the same laws that govern spatial
gestalts. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Gestalt law of proximity -- close or contiguous elements are treated as parts of the same
configuration, whereas distant elements are not. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
What gives cohesion to the gestalt
of action is not only
a temporal proximity of the individual acts that constitute it, but also their goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Perceptual
and motor acts are intertwined in the perception--behavior cycle to form
together the gestalt of
action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Central representation of gestalt of action is the equivalent of
what many researchers call the "schema." |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Novel schemas,
plans, and programs are represented in executive
cognits -- that is, in large-scale
networks of premotor
and prefrontal cortex that cross over several domains of action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Biologically critical action domain
in the prefrontal
cortex that does not appear to be hierarchically
organized -- domain for emotion. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Emotion domain of the prefrontal
cortex extends mainly through the medial and orbital aspects. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Two major foci of emotional representations within the prefrontal cortex -- (1) orbital cortex, and (2) anterior
cingulate cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Orbital cortex is intimately and reciprocally connected with the limbic structures, especially the amygdala, the hypothalamus, and the monoaminergic
systems of the brainstem. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Orbitofrontal networks collect diverse visceral inputs, as well as inputs conveying information
related to basic drives, general states of the organism, and the motivational significance of sensory stimuli. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
It is mainly in orbitofrontal cortex that information about actual and expected rewards is collected
-- through the dopaminergic system -- and funneled to the rest of the prefrontal cortex to drive and shape behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Orbital action domain is critically involved in emotion in two major ways --
(1) by acting on the cognitive networks of the cortical convexity to promote reward seeking behavior, and (2) by acting
upon subcortical structures (the nucleus accumbens, hypothalamus, striatatum,
etc.) and the autonomic and endocrine systems to support and control the major drives of the organism. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
A second major focus of the prefrontal emotional domain is the anterior cingulate cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
345 |
|
Anterior cingulate cortex is an important node in a cortical network that is involved
in attention (especially effortful attention), reward, and success or
failure to obtain reward. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Executive functions |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Executive Attention |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Executive functions of the prefrontal cortex use as physiological substrate the actions domains of the executive networks linking the domains. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Working memory integrates inputs from many cortical and subcortical regions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Working memory is at least as widely distributed as the extent of inputs represented in prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Executive functions appear to
have certain foci of dominants in the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Major cognitive functions -- attention,
perception, memory, intelligence, language |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Drive is
the source of alertness
or general attention,
and of interest in the world and in the self. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Drive determines the an
initiative and vigor with which the organism performs behavioral actions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Drive is
provided to the frontal cortex in the form of modulating activity from the subcortical and limbic structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Structures contributing to drive include, most prominently,
the reticular formation
of the mesencephalon, amygdala, hypothalamus, and the monoaminergic systems of the brain stem. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Bulk of all voluntary and deliberate behavior consists of simple and automatic acts, old habits, and familiar percepts, most of it
integrated at hierarchically lower cortices and basal ganglia. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
346 |
|
Attentive acts that "palpate" the
environment in search of significant clues, the intentional
and elaborate movements, the continuous
monitoring and updating of relevant information. [Kalman filter] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
First and most important executive function is attention. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Attention
was aptly defined by William James (1890). |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Attention
is the selective allocation of limited neural resources to the optimal processing of neural information. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Sensory attention and motor attention -- at higher levels
there is perceptual
attention and executive attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Highest form
of selective attention devoted to the active pursuit of a cognitive or behavioral goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Attention
has two complementary components -- an intensive, selective component, and an exclusionary one. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Selective
component of attention seems based primarily in dorsolateral frontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Exclusionary
component of attention, the inhibitory control of interference, seems primarily based in ventral frontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
A ubiquitously
active cognitive function such as attention cannot be
localized in any
particular brain structure. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Focus of executive attention would shift from one domain of action to another as different networks or their parts excite one another in the perception-action cycle. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
"supervisory attentional
system" |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Three major aspects of executive attention -- preparatory
set, working memory, and interference
control. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
347 |
|
Actions may
involve the executive representation (cognits) of high order, in the form of schemas, temporal gestalts, scripts,
or rules of action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
Attentive set for the
coordination of actions within a large goal directed schema involves, in
all likelihood, the top-down processing through successive
layers of cognits in the frontal
executive hierarchy. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
Set begins
at high, prefrontal, levels of the hierarchy that encode rules, plans, and long-term goals. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
Set
progresses through
lower, premotor and motor levels that encode more concrete actions for the attainment
of partial goals
toward major goals. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
Like the goals and cognits in the hierarchical
organization of
actions in frontal
cortex, partial sets are functionally nested
within larger set. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
In the organization of complex, goal directed behavior, a cascade of activation from higher frontal networks to lower ones, with monitoring and correction action at every step. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
348 |
|
Motor attention, like perceptual attention, has an exclusionary component. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
349 |
|
Working memory can be best understood as attention focused on an internal representation. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
349 |
|
Close relationship between working memory and attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
349 |
|
Working memory is indeed a form of attention -- sustained attention focused on an executive cognitive
network for the processing
of perspective action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
349 |
|
Working memory consists largely of updated
long-term memory; working memory can be
legitimately called active memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
353 |
|
Planning |
|
4 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
355 |
|
Decision-making |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
356 |
|
Emotional behavior |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
357 |
|
Temporal organization of action |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
Perception-Action Cycle |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
In all forms of behavior, motor action is not only initiated by new or
unexpected sensory signals, but also regulated by sensory feedback generated by
changes that the action
itself induces in the external
environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
"Gestalts cycle" -- union of perception and movement in the nervous system. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
Feedback from prefrontal to posterior cortices is most likely at the
root of what has been termed the cognitive control of attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
A sensation-action
cycle can be recognized at every level of the neural hierarchies for sensation and movement, from the spinal cord upwards. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
358 |
|
Cortical anatomy of the perception cycle is made of two hierarchies of cognitive networks, one perceptual in the posterior cortex and the other executive in the frontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
359 |
|
Corresponding areas of the two hierarchies, sensory and motor, on each side of the central sulcus are connected by reciprocal
connections. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
359 |
|
Each successive area of the posterior cortical pathways for three major sensory modalities -- somatosensory, vision, and audition -- send collateral efferent connections to
a progressively more rostral frontal area; all such
connections are reciprocated by others in the opposite
direction. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
359 |
|
Progressively higher stages of perceptual memory and
processing
reach progressively higher stages of executive memory and
processing and, vice
versa, reciprocal
connections flow in an orderly manner from the motor to the perceptual hierarchy. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
Perception--Action Cycle block diagram |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
Cortical connectivity apparatus of the perception-action
cycle is completed in
both directions
at every hierarchical level. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
Both perceptual and executive networks receive inputs from, and send outputs to, a number of subcortical structures that exchange information with cortical networks. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
Some of the
cortical inputs and outputs to subcortical structures
course through the
thalamus, others are
direct. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
The more critical
inputs to the cortex are those that come from the limbic
system and the hypothalamus, conveying to the prefrontal cortex information
regarding the internal
environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
The more critical
outputs from the cortex to subcortical structures
are those flowing to the basal ganglia, the cerebellum, and lower
components of the pyramidal
system. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
Some subcortical inputs and outputs constitute loops of connection through the prefrontal cortex, thus forming the
framework for an emotional perception-action cycle
intertwined and cooperating with
the cognitive cycle. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
360 |
|
To understand the structure and synthetic functions of
the frontal cortex in behavior, and its role in the perception-action cycle, it is helpful to conceptualize all behavior as a hierarchical order of structured units of sensation and action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
Afferents and Efferents of Perception--Action Cycle - (diagram) |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
A reflex
act will fit the role
as a basic cybernetic unit of interaction with the environment. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
Spinal reflex arcs, at the bottom of the sensation-action cycle may serve
as an example of the basic unit of behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
At higher
neural levels
are representations of learned behaviors, stacked roughly by order of increasing complexity. [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
Still in phyletic
memory, at diencephalic
level, are the complex
instinctual sequences, which are also modifiable
by experience and influenced
from higher levels. [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
In the basal
ganglia and the cerebellum, we encounter
representations of learned and automatic actions, which at one time may have depended on the cerebral
cortex,
but are now relegated to lower levels of sensory-motor integration. [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
At the level of the cerebral cortex, the representations of sensation (now perception) and of action associated with it become more
complex and more dependent on new plans or recent experience. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
Actions
integrated in frontal
cortex are less
automatic, more subject to deliberation. These trends increase as we go up the frontal hierarchy, from motor cortex, to premotor cortex, to prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
At high
levels of prefrontal
cortex,
actions need not be represented in all their complexity. Only the schema and the goal of a behavioral sequence need to
be there in abstract form. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
361 |
|
We have a hierarchy
of behaviors
of increasing duration and complexity serving a
corresponding hierarchy of purposes. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Supporting the execution of behaviors
is a corresponding hierarchy of neural
structures mutually engaged at various levels in the perception-action cycle. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
At all levels of the perception-action cycle hierarchy, the same networks that represent the action engage in its execution. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Feedback signals to the prefrontal cortex may come from the anterior
cingulate cortex or from other parts of the prefrontal cortex. Other feedback
inputs may come from the brainstem or the limbic system. Others may come from posterior cortical networks of perception. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Whether a structure
of action is part of the perception-action cycle or has its
origin in the prefrontal cortex, its representational
network will successively
activate a series of subnetworks representing the component actions of that structure. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
If processing begins with an action schema in prefrontal cortex, the activation will progress through premotor cortex and ultimately the primary motor cortex, where the "microgenesis"
of the action takes place. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
While the
processing of actions
generally occurs in a downward and feedforward fashion through the executive hierarchy, it necessitates the continued feedback from each level to its precursor levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Feedback
also allows the persistence of traces of sensory information in working
memory to guide the
action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Spoken language exemplifies the most highly differentiated form of the perception-action cycle. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
For spoken
language, two neocortical areas are critical -- Wernike's area and Broca's area. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
362 |
|
Higher frontal areas also needed for elaborate language. In propositional language, as in mathematical
or logical reasoning, the prefrontal
cortex seems to play a
pivotal role. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
363 |
|
Extending to the temporal domain the concepts that Gestalt psychology generally applies to the spatial
domain. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
363 |
|
The behavioral
structures for which prefrontal
cortex appears to be so important consists of novel,
usually complex, temporal gestalts. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
363 |
|
The biological
meaning of any Gestalt, spatial or temporal, lies not in its component parts but in the associative
relationships of the parts
to each other -- which naturally, in the case of temporal gestalts, include order and timing. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
The most plausible mechanism of working memory is reverberation by reentry. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Sustained activation of prefrontal cells in working
memory results, at least in part, from continued
reactivation of
cortical pyramidal cells through reentrant circuits, whether those circuits are local or course through structures outside the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Persistent activation of cortical neurons in working
memory can be explained as a phenomenon of reentry in existing cognitive networks of
long-term memory with synaptic weights pre-established
by learning. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Set is the
other major temporal-integration function of the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Set is
conventionally understood as a preparation for
action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
In several respects, set is the opposite of working memory. Whereas the content of working memory is
commonly sensory, that of set is motor or action-oriented. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Whereas working memory is
attention directed to an internal representation, set is attention directed to a prospective
action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
366 |
|
Speech, internal action, and logical reasoning all need set, in addition to working memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
367 |
|
Working memory and set
are the basis of what has been termed cognitive
control of the prefrontal
cortex. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
368 |
|
Language |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
369 |
|
Creative Intelligence |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
371 |
|
Alternative Models |
|
2 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
376 |
|
Consciousness and Free Will |
|
5 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
379 |
|
One neuron or group of neurons can be part of many networks, and thus many representations of memory, perception, or action. |
|
3 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
379 |
|
One neuron or group of neurons can serve several cognitive functions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
379 |
|
Prefrontal cortex of the dorsolateral aspects of the frontal lobe is devoted to the cognitive
functions that control the execution of goal-directed actions. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
379 |
|
Prefrontal cortex performs its cognitive role in cooperation with orbitomedial and posterior association cortices,
the striatatum, and
other subcortical structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
In the primate, the cerebral cortex of both hemispheres is divided into two major sectors by the central sulcus. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Posterior sector of the cortical hemispheres is dedicated to sensation,
perception, and
perceptual memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Frontal sector
of the cortical
hemispheres is dedicated to action and executive memory. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Representational networks or cognits of the two cortical regions, posterior and frontal, are hierarchically
organized by development and connectivity. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
The three
principal executive functions of the prefrontal cortex are (1) executive
attention, (2) planning, (3) decision-making. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Executive
attention is the first major executive function of the prefrontal cortex, and has three
aspects or subfunctions -- (1) set, (2) working
memory,
(3) interference control. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Set consist
in the selection of
particular motor acts and the anticipatory preparation of the sensory and motor systems for
them. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Set is
based in lateral prefrontal cortex, and operates on its executive
networks and through lower
executive structures. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Working memory is attention focused on an internal
representation for a purposive
action in the proximate
future. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Working memory is the ad hoc temporary retention of an updated long-term memory for prospective action. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Working memory consists in the sustained
temporary activation of an executive cognitive network of the cerebral cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Working memory
is active by reverberation through reentrant circuits. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
380 |
|
Interference control is the exclusionary or suppressive
aspect of executive
attention. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Interference control protects behavioral structures from
external or internal interference. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Interference control is an inhibitory
function based primarily in orbitomedial prefrontal cortex, It
exerts its influence on a variety of orbital and subcortical regions, prominently the basal ganglia. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Planning is the second major executive
function of the prefrontal
cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is essential for the formulation
and execution of novel
plans or structures
(gestalts) of goal directed behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Gestalts of action with their goals are represented in neuronal networks in the form of abstract schemas. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Simpler components of
structures of actions (sub-plans and sub-goals)
are represented in frontal or subcortical networks at lower levels of the
motor hierarchies. [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Orderly arrangement of fiber
connections links the various stages of the motor
hierarchies in lateral
frontal cortex to support the execution of action plans. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Connectivity
in the lateral frontal cortex generally flows downward from prefrontal, to premotor, to motor cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
All stages
within each action domain in the lateral frontal cortex are reciprocally connected and, in addition, are connected
with each other via subcortical
loops through the basal
ganglia. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
In the processing of sequential action in the lateral frontal cortex, parallel as well as serial processing takes place within and between action
domains. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Decision
making is the third major executive function of the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
A deliberate
decision to perform an
action is a multi-determinate
phenomena, a vector
of numerous and diverse neural influences that converge on frontal cortex from other brain regions and from the cortex itself. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Foremost among the factors influencing a decision for action are the basic drive and motivations of the organism. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Signals of the basic drive and motivations of the
organism arrive in prefrontal cortex via the orbitofrontal cortex, the diencephalon, and limbic formations. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
In making the
final decision to perform an act or to undertake
a course of action, there is an estimation of the valence of the relevant stimuli and of the costs and benefits of the action to be taken. [Bayesian inference] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Costs and benefits of the action to be taken most likely involves probabilistic
computations conducted in prefrontal cortex based on current inputs and past history. [Bayesian inference] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Among the signals collected by
the prefrontal system are
those from the anterior cingulate cortex, which monitors success or failure of past actions. [Bayesian inference] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
381 |
|
Orbitomedial prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role in emotional
behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Orbital medial cortex, through
its efferent fibers to lateral prefrontal cortex, influences cognitive
executive functions. |
|
1 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Afferent influences from lateral
to orbitomedial cortex
exerts a degree of cognitive
control over emotional
behavior. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Functions of the prefrontal cortex join together
synergistically to achieve the temporal
organization of action, i.e. the orderly sequencing of
actions toward a goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
The supra-ordinate function of temporal organization, consisting
of a joint operation of all cognitive and emotional functions of the prefrontal cortex, is of fundamental importance not only to behavior but also
in reasoning and language. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
To understand the neural dynamics of temporal organization, the prefrontal cortex and its functions must be viewed within the biological
framework of the perception-action cycle. [Bayesian
inference] [Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Perception-action cycle is a circular cybernetic flow of
information processing between the organism and its environment in a sequence of goal-directed actions. [Bayesian inference] [Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
An action of the organism causes
an environmental change that will be
processed by sensory systems, which will produce signals to inform the next
action, and so on. [Bayesian inference] [Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Perception-action cycle is of prime importance for the adaptive success of a temporally extended gestalt of behavior, where each action is contingent on the effects of the previous one. [Bayesian inference] [Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Perception-action cycle operates
at all levels of the central nervous system.
[Bayesian inference]
[Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Simple, automatic, and well
rehearsed behaviors engage only the lower levels of the perception-action
cycle, where, for sensorimotor integration, the cycle runs through the spinal
cord and subcortical structures. [Stereotyped
motor programs] [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Complex, novel, and temporally
extended behaviors, engage the neocortex and the connections between the
prefrontal and posterior association cortex.
[Bayesian inference] [Recursion] |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Prefrontal cortex sits at the summit of the perception-action cycle,
integrating across time percepts and actions toward a goal. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Language
engages the perception action cycle at its highest levels. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
At the highest levels,
cross-temporal contingencies are mediated by the reciprocal interactions
between frontal cortex, including Broca's area, and posterior association
cortex, including Wernike's area. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Working memory plays a key role in the underlying operations of temporal integration. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
382 |
|
Creative intelligence, the ability to construct new forms of action and expression,
undergoes enormous expansion in the human as a result of the phylogenetic and
ontogenetic development of the prefrontal cortex. |
|
0 |
Fuster; Prefrontal Cortex |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|