Jeff Hawkins - On Intelligence
Book Page   Topic            
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 In an auto associative memory you don't have to have the entire pattern you want to  retrieve in order to retrieve it. #####
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 You might have only part of the pattern, or you might have a somewhat messed up pattern 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 The auto-associative memory can retrieve the correct pattern, as it was originally stored, even though you start with a messy version of it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 Going to a bank with a ripped and unreadable currency bill and the banker says "I think this is a messed up $100 bill. Give me that one, and I will give you this nice crisp $100 bill. “ 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 An auto associative memory can be designed to store sequences of patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 With a sequence of patterns, similar to a portion of a melody, the auto associative memory can remember the entire melody. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 30 You might enter the first few notes of "twinkle twinkle little star" and the auto associative memory returns the whole song. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 31 People learn practically everything as a sequence of patterns. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 65 Your brain is being flooded    with the spatial and temporal patterns    from all of your senses. 34
Hawkins - On Intelligence 66 A neuron collects inputs from  its synapses,    and combines these inputs together    to decide when to output a spike    to other neurons. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 66 A typical neuron can cycle its functions and reset itself in about 5 ms, or around 200 times per second. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 66 The brain is a parallel computer.    It has billions of cells    all computing at the same time. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 68 The brain does not compute    the answers to problems.    It retrieves the answers    from memory. 2
Hawkins - On Intelligence 68 The essence the answers    were stored in memory    a long time ago. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 68 The entire cortex is a memory system.    It is not a computer at all. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 69 A retrieved memory    is adjusted as it is recalled    to accommodate    the particulars of the moment. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 69 The memory of how to catch a ball    was not programmed into your brain;    it was learned over years of repetitive practice,   and it is stored,    not calculated,   in your neurons. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 69 The neocortex is not like a computer,    parallel or otherwise.    Instead of computing answers to problems,    the neocortex uses stored memories    to solve problems and produce behavior. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 Neocortex stores sequences of patterns. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 Neocortex    recalls patterns    auto-associatively. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 Neocortex    stores patterns    in an invariant form. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 Neocortex    stores patterns    in a hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 A story    is stored in your head    in a sequential fashion    and can only be recalled    in the same sequence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 It's almost impossible    to think of anything complex    that isn't a series of events or thoughts. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 In telling a story    some people can't get to the point of it right away.    They seem to ramble on   with irritating details and tangents. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 70 They are promulgating the story    as it happened to them,    through time,    and cannot tell it any other way. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 71 All memories are like this.    You have to walk through the temporal sequence    of how you do things. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 71 Your memory    of the alphabet    is a sequence of patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 71 The same thing is goals for the days of the week for the months of the year, your phone number, your countless other things. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 71 Your memory for songs    is a great example    of temporal sequences and memory. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 72 You can't recall a song backward. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 72 Tactile memory for textures. Your memory for the tactical memory of gravel is based on pattern sequences across the pressure and vibration sensing neurons of your skin. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 All memories    are stored in the synaptic connections    between neurons. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 Only a limited number of synapses and neurons in your skin    are playing an active role in memory recall    at any one time. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 An adult human neocortex    has an incredibly large memory capacity    but we can only remember a few    at any time    and can only do so    in a sequence of association. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 There are thousands of detailed memories    stored in the synapses of our brains    that are rarely used. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 Most of the information in our brains that we know    is sitting there idly    waiting for the appropriate cues to invoke it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 Autoassociative nature of memory. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 Patterns    associated with themselves. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 An autoassociative memory    is one that can recall complete patterns    when given only a partial    or distorted input. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 73 Autoassociative memory can work    for both spatial    and temporal patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 If you recall a small detail    about something that happened long ago,    the entire memory sequence    can come flooding back into your mind. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 During conversation    we often can't hear all the words    we are in a noisy environment.    Our brains fill in    what they miss    with what they expect to hear. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 It is well established that we don't actually hear    all the sounds    we perceive. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 Some people    complete of the sentences    of others aloud,    but in our minds    all of us    are doing this constantly. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 For the most part we are not aware    that we are constantly completing patterns,    but it's a ubiquitous and fundamental feature    of how memories are stored in the cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 At any time,    a piece    can activate    the whole.    This is the essence of auto associated memories. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 Your neocortex    is a complex biological autoassociative memory. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 During each waking moment,    each functional region    of your neocortex    is essentially waiting vigilantly    for familiar patterns    or pattern fragments. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 74 The mere appearance of your friend    forces your brain    to start recalling patterns    associated with her. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 82 Memories are stored    in a form that captures the essence of relationships,    not the details of the moment. 8
Hawkins - On Intelligence 82 When you see, feel, or hear something,    the cortex takes the detailed highly specific input   and converts it    to an invariant form. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 82 It is the invariant form    that is stored in memory,    and it is the invariant form    of each new input pattern    that it gets compared to. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 82 Memory storage,    memory recall,    and memory recognition    occur at the level of invariant forms. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 82 An important function    of the neocortex    is to use its memory    to make predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 83 When you see your friends face,    your cortex    fills in and predicts    the myriad details. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 83 Your cortex    makes these predictions    with great specificity. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 84 Your brain does this    by combining a memory    of the invariant structure of her face    with the particulars of your immediate experience. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 84 The combining    of invariant representations    and current input    to make detailed predictions    is exactly what is happening. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 84 This combining    is a ubiquitous process    that happens in every region of  cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 84 We are able to predict    not only the words others will say,    but also in what tone of voice    they will say them. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 84 Three properties of cortical memory    (storing sequences,    autoassociative recall,    and invariant representations)    are necessary ingredients to predict the future    based on memories of the past. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 86 Our brains use stored memories    to constantly make predictions    about everything we see,    feel,    and hear 2
Hawkins - On Intelligence 86 The majority of predictions    occur outside of awareness. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 87 Brain constantly makes predictions    about the very fabric of the world we live in. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 87 The predictions    are made in parallel    and will just as readily detect    an odd texture,    a misshapen nose,    or an unusual motion. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 87 What we perceive    is a combination    of what we sense    and of our brain's memory=derived predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 88 All regions of the neocortex    are simultaneously trying to predict    what their next experience will be. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 88 Visual areas make predictions about edges,    shapes,    objects,    locations,    and motions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 88 Auditory areas make predictions about tones,    direction to source,    and patterns of sound. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 89 somatosensory areas make predictions about touch,    texture,    contour,    and temperature. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 89 Correct predictions result in understanding. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 89 Prediction    is the primary function of the neocortex,    and the foundation of intelligence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 90 Rodolfo Llinas, I of the Vortex, the capacity to predict the outcome of future events is most likely, the ultimate and most common of all global brain functions. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 90 There is an entire subfield of mathematics devoted to Bayesian networks. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 90 Bayesian networks use probability theory to make predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 91 Prediction is pervasive    and the basis for how you understand the world. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 91 When you listen to a favorite melody,    you hear the next note in your head    before it occurs. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 92 Neurons in your head will fire when you hear that next out fire in advance of your actual hearing it, and so you hear this song in your head. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 92 When listening to people speak,    you often know what they're going to say     before they finished speaking. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 92 People tend to use     common phrases or expressions    in much of their conversation. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 92 Prediction   is not always exact.    Rather, our minds work to make probabilistic predictions    concerning what is about to happen. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 92 At times,    our expectations    are distributed   among several  possibilities 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 93 In Western music,    the brain automatically predicts    beats,    repeated rhythms,    completion of phrases,    and end of songs. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 93 The basis    of these mostly unconscious predictions    is a set of memories    that are stored in your cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 93 Your brain can't say    exactly what will happen next,    but it nevertheless predicts which note  patterns    are likely to happen    and which aren't. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 93 We see    what we expect to see. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 97 Intelligence    is measured by the capacity to remember and predict    patterns in the world    including language,    mathematics,    physical properties of objects,    and social situations. 4
Hawkins - On Intelligence 97 The brain    receives patterns from the outside world,    stores them in memories,    and makes predictions    by combining    what it has seen before    and what is happening now. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 The human cortex    is the same thickness    and is very nearly the same structure    as the cortex of our mammal relatives. 2
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 When evolution makes something big     very quickly,    as it did with the human cortex,    it does so by copying an existing structure. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 Humans got smart    by adding many more elements    of a common cortical algorithm. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 The human neocortex    is a relatively new structure. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 Memory and prediction    are the keys    to unlocking the mystery of intelligence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 Start with the reptilian brain with no cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 99 If evolution tacks on a memory system (neocortex) to the sensory path of the primitive brain, the animal gains and ability to predict the future. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 103 The back part of the cortex    contains the sections where the eyes,    ears,    and touch    inputs arrived. 4
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 Reptiles with sophisticated senses and sophisticated but relatively rigid behaviors. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 Nature discovered that by adding a memory system    and feeding the sensory stream into it,    the animal could remember past experiences. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 When the animal found itself in the same or similar situation, the memory will be recall, leading to a prediction of what was likely to happen next. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 Intelligence and understanding    started as a memory system    that fed predictions into the sensory stream. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 To know something means that you can make predictions about it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 With humans,    the cortex has taken over most of the motor behavior. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 104 Instead of just making predictions based on behavior of the old brain.    The human neocortex    directs behavior    to satisfy the prediction. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 105 To make predictions of future events,    the neocortex must store sequences of patterns. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 105 To recall the appropriate memories,    the brain has to retrieve patterns    by their similarity to past patterns (auto associative recall). 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 105 Memories have to be stored in an invariant form    so that the knowledge of past events    can be applied to new situations    that are similar but not identical    to the past. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 Information also flows    from higher to lower regions    via a network of feedback connections. 8
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 As many if not more feedback connections in visual cortex as there are feedforward connections. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 The cortex's core function is to make predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 Prediction requires a comparison    between what is happening    and what you expect to happen. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 What is actually happening flows up, and what you expect to happen flows down. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 113 The same feedforward=feedback process    is occurring in all your cortical areas    involving all your senses. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 114 The transformation    from fast-changing to slow changing    and from spatially specific to spatially invariant,    is well documented for vision. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 115 For hearing, when someone speaks to you, the changes in sound pressure occur very rapidly; the patterns entering the primary auditory area change just as rapidly. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 115 Patterns received by the first auditory area    can vary widely.    A word can be spoken    with different accents,    and different pitches, or at different speeds. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 115 Higher up in the cortex,     those low level features don't matter;     a word is a word    regardless of the acoustic details. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 115 We see the same kind of feedback,    prediction,    and invariant recall    in auditory cortex    as we see in the visual system. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 116 There are very different inputs arriving at different locations in the primary battle centers somatosensory cortex. Again we would find selves and regions several steps removed from the primary input that respond to an object. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 Information    flows up and down    sensory areas of the cortex. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 The downward flow    fills in the current input    and makes predictions   about what we will experience next. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 Something I hear    can lead to a prediction    of what I should see or feel. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 Information flows up    the auditory hierarchy    to an association area    that connects vision with hearing. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 The representation then flows back down     the auditory and visual hierarchies,    leading to both auditory    and visual predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 117 This kind of     multisensory prediction    is occurring all the time. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 118 Information    simultaneously flows up and down    the sensory hierarchies    to create a unified sensory experience    involving prediction in all senses. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 All the little sensations    are fully integrated    into our perceptual predictions. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 These predictions    can only come about    by massive coordination    of patterns streaming up and down    the cortical hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 The entire neocortex,    all the sensory and association areas,    act as one. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 We have an overarching sensory system,    sights,    sounds,    touch,    and more    combined,    all flowing up and down    a single multi-branched hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 All predictions    are learned by experience. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 119 You are not born with    any of this knowledge;    you learned it through the incredibly large capacity    of your cortex    to remember patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 If there are consistent patterns    among the inputs flowing into your brain,    your cortex will use them    to predict future events. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 An input in a sensory area    can flow to an association area    which can lead to    a pattern flowing down the motor cortex    resulting in behavior. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 We interpret    these downward flowing patterns    as predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 In the motor cortex    we interpret the downward flowing patterns    as motor commands. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 As Mountcastle pointed out,    the motor cortex    looks like    the sensory cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 The way the cortex processes    downward-flowing sensory predictions    is similar to    how it processes    downward-flowing motor commands. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 There are    no pure sensory    or pure motor areas    in the cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 Sensory patterns    simultaneously flow in anywhere and everywhere,    and then flow back down    in any area of the hierarchy,    leading to predictions or motor behavior. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 Although the motor cortex    has some special attributes,    it is correct to think of it    as just part of one large hierarchical memory-prediction system. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 120 Seeing,    hearing,    touching,    and acting    are profoundly intertwined. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 V1,V2, and V4,    each is a collection    of many smaller subregions. 2
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 The largest region by far    is V1,    the primary visual area. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 Next would be V2.    They are large    compared to most regions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 V1 is made up of numerous separate little cortical areas     that are only connected to their neighbors indirectly,    through regions higher up in the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 V1 would have    the largest number of small subregions    of any visual area. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 V2 would be composed    of fewer,    slightly larger subregions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 The same would be true    for V4. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 122 The top region IT    would have only a single region,    which has a birds eye view    of the entire visual world. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 The cortex    now looks similar    everywhere. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 Pick any region    and you will find many lower regions    providing converging sensory input. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 The receiving region    sends projections back    to the input regions,    telling them what patterns    they should expect to see next. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 Higher association areas    unite information    from multiple senses    such as vision and touch. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 A lower region    like a subregion of V2    unites the information    from separate subregions    within V1. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 A region doesn't know – indeed it can't know – what any of those inputs mean. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 An association area    doesn't need to know    that it is handling visual input    from multiple pairs of V1. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 An association area    doesn't need to know    that it is handling input    from vision and hearing. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 123 The job of any cortical region    is to find out how its inputs are related,    to memorize the sequence of correlations between them,    and to use its memory    to predict how the inputs    will behave in the future. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 The same process    is happening everywhere:     a common cortical algorithm. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 This hierarchical depiction    helps us understand    the process of creating invariant representations. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 The most important result    of this depiction of cortical hierarchy    is that every region of cortex    forms invariant representations. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 Now we can say that invariant representations are ubiquitous.    Invariant representations are formed in every cortical region. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 Every region    forms invariant representations    drawn from the input areas    hierarchically below it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 124 Thus the subregions of V4, V2, and V1    create invariant representations    based on what flows into them. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 125 Association regions    above IT    form invariant representations    of patterns from multiple senses. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 125 All regions of the cortex    form invariant representations    of the world beneath them in the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 125 Why is the neocortex built as a hierarchy?    Because the cortex    has built a model of the world. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 125 The cortex's hierarchical structure    stores a model    of the hierarchical structure of the real world. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 125 The real world's nested structure   is mirrored    by the nested structure of the cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 126 Patterns from the retina    entering your primary visual cortex    are being combined    to form components of visual objects. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 126 The function of the cortex and the method by which it learns naturally discover the hierarchical relationships in the world. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 127 You are not born with    knowledge of language,    houses,    or music. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 127 The cortex has a clever learning algorithm     that naturally finds    whatever hierarchical structure exists    and captures it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 127 Higher regions of the cortex    are keeping track of the big picture    while lower areas are actively dealing with the fast-changing small details. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 127 Since we can only touch,    hear,    and see    a very small part of the world     any moment in time,    information flowing into the brain    naturally arise as a sequence of patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 127 The cortex functions to learn     those sequences    that occur over and over again. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 128 Each region of cortex    sees a stream of patterns. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 128 If the patterns are related    in such a way    that the region can learn to predict    what pattern will occur next,    the cortical region    forms a persistent representation,   or memory,    for the sequence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 128 Learning sequences    is the most basic ingredient    for forming invariant representations    of real-world objects. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 128 Predictability    is the very definition of reality. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 The brain can be said to store sequences of sequences.    Each region of the cortex    learns the sequences,      develops what Hawkins calls “names” for the  sequences it knows,    and passes these names      to the next regions    higher in the cortical hierarchy. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 As information moves up    from primary sensory regions    to higher levels,    we see fewer and fewer    changes over time. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 In primary visual areas like V1,    the set of active cells    is changing rapidly    as new patterns fall on the retina    several times each second. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 In visual area IT,    self firing patterns    are more stable. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 Each region of cortex    has a repertoire    of sequences it knows. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 Regions    store  these sequences    about anything and everything. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 Each cortical region    has a name    for each sequence    it knows. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 The "name"    is a group of cells    whose collected firing    represents the set of objects    in the sequence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 129 These cells remain active    as long as the sequences playing,    and it is this “name”    that is passed up    to the next region in the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 As long as the input patterns    are part of a predictable sequence,    the region presents a constant “name”    to the next higher region. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 We can imagine region IT    at the top of the visual hierarchy    relaying to an association area above it, "I am seeing a face." 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 In this way, a predictable    sequence of events    gets identified    with a "name." 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 This happens over and over again    as we go up    in the hierarchical pyramid. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 One region    might recognize a sequence of sounds    that comprise phonograms. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 The next higher region    recognizes sequences the phonems    to create words. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 The next higher region    recognizes sequences of words    to create phrases,    and so on. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 By collapsing predictable sequences    into named objects    at each region in the hierarchy,    we achieve more and more stability    the higher we go. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 This creates invariant representation. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 130 The opposite effect happens    as a pattern moves back down the hierarchy:    stable patterns    get unfolded into sequences. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 At this point the unfolding pattern splits    and travels down both the auditory section of cortex    and the motor section of cortex. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 Following the motor path,    each word is unfolded    into a memorized sequence of phonems. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 In the final bottom region,    each phonem is unfolded    into a sequence of muscle commands    to make sounds. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 The lower you look in the hierarchy,    the faster the patterns are changing. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 A single constant pattern    at the top of the motor hierarchy    eventually leads to    a complex and lengthy sequence    of speech sounds. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 If you want to type the Gettysburg address    you start with the same pattern    at the top of the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 Words are unfolded into letters,    and the letters are unfolded into muscle commands    to your fingers    for typing. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 A single memory of the speech    can take various behavioral forms. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 At any region,    an invariant pattern    can bifurcate    and follow a different path down. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 Representations of simple objects    at the bottom of the hierarchy    can be reused over and over    for different high-level sequences. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 131 A hierarchy of nested sequences    allows the sharing and reuse    of lower-level objects words,    phonems,    and letters    being  but a few examples. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 132 It is a remarkably efficient way    to store information about the world and its structure    and very different    from how computers work. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 132 The same unfolding of sequences    occurs in the sensory    as well as the motor regions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 132 The unfolding of sequences    allows you to perceive and understand objects    from different views. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 132 The way you memorize sequences    and represent them by name    as information goes up and down the cortical hierarchy    may remind you of the hierarchy of military command. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 If something goes wrong that cannot be handled by subordinates down the chain of command, then the issue rises up the hierarchy until someone knows  what to do next. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 What was an unanticipated problem to subordinates is just the expected next task for the officer in charge. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 If lower regions of cortex    failed to predict what patterns they are seeing,    they consider that an error    and pass the error up the hierarchy.     This is repeated    until some region does anticipate the pattern. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 Every cortical region    attempts to    store and recall    sequences. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 The bottom-up inputs to a region of cortex    are input patterns    carried on thousands or millions of axons. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 These axons    come from different regions    and contain    all sorts of patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 The number of possible patterns    that can exist on even 1000 axons    is larger than    the number of molecules in the universe. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 133 A region will only see a tiny fraction of these possible patterns in a lifetime. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 134 The brain    must classify patterns. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 134 You are going to look at all the input patterns    coming in from lower cortical regions,     classify them,    and then look for sequences.    0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 134 Both steps,    classification and sequence formation,    are necessary    to create invariant representations,     and each region of cortex    does them. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 If you know    the most likely sequence    for a series of inputs,    you will use this knowledge    to decide    how to classify    the ambiguous input. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 You use the context of known sequences    to resolve ambiguity 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 When people speak    their individual words    very often cannot be understood    out of context. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 Most of the time    you are aware    that you are filling in    ambiguous or incomplete information    from your memories or sequence    0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 You hear what you expect to hear and see what you expect to see – at least when what you hear and see fits into past experience. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 Memory of sequences allows you not only to resolve ambiguity and current input, but also to predict which input should happen next. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 By recognizing a sequence of patterns,    a cortical region will predict its next input pattern    and tell the region below    what to expect. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 135 A region of cortex    not only learns familiar sequences,    it also learns how    to modify its classifications. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 136 In cortical regions,    bottom-up classifications    and top-down sequences    are constantly interacting,    changing throughout your life. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 136 This is the essence of learning. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 136 Forming new classifications    and new sequences    is how you remember the world. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 136 Another part of the cortical job    is to relay the name of the sequence you are seeing    to the next level up. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 The hierarchy of the cortex    ensures that memories of objects are distributed over the hierarchy;    they aren't located in a single spot. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 Because each region of the hierarchy    forms invariant memories,    what a typical region of cortex learns is sequences of invariant. representations,    which are themselves sequences of invariant memories 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 When you think about the world you are recalling sequences of patterns that correspond to the way the objects of the world are and how they behave. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 The order in which you experience parts of the world is determined by the world structure. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 An invariant representation in any region of the cortex can be turned into a detailed prediction of how it will appear on yours senses by propagating the pattern down the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 137 Similarly, an invariant representation in the motor cortex can be turned into detailed and situation specific motor commands by propagating the pattern down the motor hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 138 We will start with a description of what a cortical region looks like. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 138 Cortical regions    vary greatly in size,    the largest being the primary sensory areas. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 138 Let's assume that a typical cortical area    is the size of a small coin. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 The density and shape    of the cells in the cortex    vary as you move from top to bottom. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 These differences    define the layers. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Layer 1, the top layer    is the most distinct of the six layers.    It has very few cells    consisting primarily of a mat of axons    running parallel to the cortical surface 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Layers 2 and 3 looks similar.    They contain many,    tightly packed. pyramidal cells. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Layer 4 has a type of star-shaped cell. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Layer 5 has regular pyramidal cells    as well as a class of extra big pyramidal-shaped cells. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Layers 6,  the bottom layer   also has several types of unique neurons. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Columns of cells    run perpendicular to the layers.    You can think of columns    as being vertical units of cells    that work together. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 The layers within each column    are connected by axons    that run up and down,    making synapses along the way. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 Columns do not have clear boundaries    but their existence can be inferred    from several lines of evidence. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 One reason is that it vertically aligned cells    in each column    tend to become active    for the same stimulus. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 139 The cells within each column    are strongly connected. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Activity spreads up and down    within a column of cells. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 In an embryo,    single precursor cells    migrate from an inner brain cavity    to where the cortex takes take shape. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Each of these cells    divides    to create about 100 neurons,    called a microcolumn. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 The human cortex    has an estimated several hundred million    microcolumns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Imagine a single microcolumn is the width of a human hair. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 The brush like mat    is a simplistic model    of the coin size cortical region. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 And information flows    mostly in the direction of the hairs:    horizontally in layer 1    and vertically in layers 2 through 6 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 At least 90% of the synapses    on cells within each column    come from places outside the column itself. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Some connections    arrived from neighboring column. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Other connections    come from halfway across the brain. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 140 Vernon Mountcastle argued there is a single cortical algorithm,    he also proposed a cortical column is the basic unit of computation in the cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 141 It is believed that the column is the basic unit of prediction. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 141 Converging inputs from lower regions always arrive at layer 4, the main input layer. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 141 Layer 4 cells then send projections up to cells in layer 2 and layer 3    within their column. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 141 Layer 6 cells are the downward projecting output cells    from a cortical column    and project to layer 1    in the region hierarchically below. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 153 The sum of all these mechanisms    allows the cortex to learn sequences,    make predictions,    and form constant representations,    or "names," for sequences. 12
Hawkins - On Intelligence 153 How does a region of the cortex    make specific predictions    from invariant memories? 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 153 We have to combine    feedforward information (actual input)    with feedback information (a prediction in an invariant form). 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 Every moment of your waking life,    each region of the cortex is comparing    a set of expected columns driven from above    with the set of observed columns driven from below. 3
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 Where the two sets intersect    is what we perceive. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 If we had perfect input from below    and perfect predictions,    then the set of perceived columns    would always be contained    in the set of predicted columns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 We often don't have such agreement.    The method of combining partial prediction    with partial input    resolves ambiguous input,    it fills in missing pieces of information,    and it decides between alternate views. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 It is how we decide whether a picture is of a vase or two faces. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 In addition to projecting to lower cortical regions,    layer 6 cells    can send their outputs back    into layer 4 cells    of their own column. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 When they do, our predictions become the input. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 This is what we do when daydreaming or thinking. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 It allows us to the consequences of our own predictions. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 156 We do this many hours a day as we plan the future, worry about events to come, or just "imagining." 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 Confusion occurs    when the cortex can't find any memory    that matches with the input. 4
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 Your eyes scan everywhere on the picture. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 New inputs race all the way up the cortical hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 High-level cortex tries lots of different hypotheses but, as these predictions raced down the hierarchy, each and every one conflicts with the input and the cortex is forced to try again. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 During this time of confusion your brain is totally occupied with understanding the picture. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 160 Finally, you make a high-level prediction that is the right one. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 161 The prediction    starts at the top of the cortical hierarchy    and succeeds in propagating all the way to the bottom. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 161 In less than a second,    each region is given a sequence    that fits the data. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 161 No more errors    rise to the top. You understand the picture, you see a Dalmatian dog. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 168 Hippocampus on top of it all 7
Hawkins - On Intelligence 168 Three large brain structures    lie under the neocortical sheet and communicate with it. They are the basal ganglia,    cerebellum,    and the hippocampus. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 168 All three existed prior to the neocortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 168 The basal ganglia were the primitive motor system,    the cerebellum learned precise timing relationships of events,    and the hippocampus stored memories of specific events and places. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 168 The neocortex is responsible for all complex motor sequences    and can directly control your limbs. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 Connections between the hippocampus and the neocortex    suggest that the hippocampus is the top region of the neocortex. 2
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 The hippocampus occupies the peak of the neocortical pyramid. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 The hippocampus not only sits at the top of the cortical pyramid,    but it still connects directly to many older parts of the brain. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 Think about the information flowing from     your eyes,    ears,    and skin    into the neocortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 Each region of the neocortex    tries to understand the input    in the in terms of the sequences it knows. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 If it does understand the input    it does not pass on the details    to higher levels of the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 If a region does not understand the current input,    it passes it up the hierarchy    until some higher region    does understand it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 A pattern that is truly novel    will escalate further and further up the hierarchy   until some higher region    does understand it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 170 The net effect is that when you get to the top of the cortical pyramid,    what you have left is information that can't be understood    by prior experience. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 You are left with    the part of the input    that is truly new and unexpected. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 It is these unexplained and unanticipated remainders,    the new stuff,    that enters the hippocampus    and is stored there. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 This new, fresh information    won't be stored forever.    Either it will be transferred into the cortex    or it will eventually be lost. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 The hippocampus has a heterogeneous structure    with several specialized regions.    It's good at the unique task of quickly storing    whatever pattern it sees. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 You can instantly remember a novel event in the hippocampus,    but you will permanently remember something in the cortex   only if you experience it over and over,    either in reality or by thinking of it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 Alternate Path up the Hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 171 The cortex has a second major pathway    for passing information    from region to region,    up the hierarchy. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 172 The alternate path starts with cells in layer 5    which project to the thalamus    and then up to the next higher region    of cortex. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 172 As we move up the cortical hierarchy,    there is a direct path    between two regions    and an indirect path    through the thalamus. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 173 The alternate pathway    through the thalamus    is likely the mechanism    by which we attend to details    that normally we would not notice. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 173 It bypasses the grouping of sequences    in layer 2,    sending the raw data to the next higher region    of cortex. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 174 In this way unusual events    quickly rise    to your attention. 1
Hawkins - On Intelligence 174 This is why   we can't avoid focusing on deformities    and other unusual patterns. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence 174 Often however, errors aren't strong enough    to open the alternate pathway.    This is why we sometimes don't notice    if a word is misspelled as we read it. 0
Hawkins - On Intelligence
Hawkins - On Intelligence
Hawkins - On Intelligence
Hawkins - On Intelligence