Allan
Hobson; Consciousness |
|
|
Book |
Page |
|
Topic |
|
|
Hobson; Consciousness |
16 |
|
Primary components
of consciousness are those experienced
by all mammals, including human infants: sensation, perception, attention, emotion, instinct, movement. |
|
|
Hobson; Consciousness |
16 |
|
Building blocks of consciousness (table) |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
17 |
|
Secondary components
of consciousness are those experienced only by adult humans -- memory,
thought, language, intention, orientation, volition. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
17 |
|
Consciousness is graded across species as they develop over
evolutionary time
(phylogenesis). |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
17 |
|
Consciousness is graded within species over each individual's lifetime (ontogenesis). |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
17 |
|
Consciousness is modulated in everyone over the course of
each 24-hour day. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
19 |
|
Roger Penrose
-- subcellular elements, microtubules, tiny capillaries within nerve cells that serve as an
internal circulatory system for proteins. [contentious theory] |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
19 |
|
The spontaneous
tendency of complex
systems to change state from chaos to self-organization is relevant to
our understanding of dream consciousness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
42 |
|
Associative
nature of human
thought. Ideas are interconnected and sequential. [Gestalts] |
|
23 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
42 |
|
Even simple
and completely nonconscious brains learn associatively. Pair one
stimulus with another in space or time, and the lowliest
creature will learn the association. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
42 |
|
Dreaming serves to loosen associations lest they become obsessively tight. Francis Crick theorized that we dream in order to forget. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
44 |
|
Essential chemical
ingredient of associative
learning in sea slugs is serotonin, a neurotransmitter molecule humans need to stay awake, attentive, and teachable. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
45 |
|
Serotonin may serve to restrain
cerebral chaos and may be a global organizer of the brain, assuring consistency and stability of conscious state during waking. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
45 |
|
All complex
systems with chaotic properties can self-organize. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
45 |
|
Self-organization is more likely to be
achieved in undirected states, such as meditation,
fantasy, or reverie, that border on waking. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
45 |
|
Dreaming may be our most creative conscious state, one in which the chaotic, spontaneous recombination of cognitive
elements produces novel
configurations of information -- new ideas. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
47 |
|
Neuromodulatory neurons of the brainstem - (diagram) |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
58 |
|
Cortex lobes - (diagram) |
|
11 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
59 |
|
Sleep-dream-wake cycle is triggered and tuned by neuronal circuits in the pons. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
59 |
|
Key brain structure involved in attention is the thalamus, a large collection of cells
located atop
the brainstem in the center of the upper brain. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
60 |
|
Hypothalamus contains the
biological clock that times the body's cycles of rest and activity and gates the sleep-wake cycle in the pons. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
61 |
|
Brain structures of emotion lie below the
thalamus and cortex and above the spinal
cord and brainstem. Taken together,
the amygdala, hippocampus, and hypothalamus have been called the limbic lobe of the brain |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
62 |
|
Intimate relationship between the hippocampus, which is essential to
memory, and the structures mediating emotion. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
62 |
|
Mental faculty of orientation cannot properly be
considered apart from memory. Knowing who one is, what day it is, and where one is. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
62 |
|
Distinguish (1) the cognitive kind of orientation (which provides an organizing set of parameters for
the rest of cognition) and (2) the instinctive
or reflexive orienting behavior that is our
immediate response (startle reflex) to a novel or
surprising stimulus that suddenly
seizes our attention. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
63 |
|
Distinction between older brain
structures like the
upper pontine brainstem, with its direct
connections to the amygdala that mediates startle responses, and the newer midbrain-limbic circuits linking the mammillary
bodies to the hippocampus, which underlie
accurate orienting in
space. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
63 |
|
Association cortex -- (diagram) |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
64 |
|
Although we are largely unaware of it, our brains
are full of implicit information about space. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
67 |
|
Level of any conscious
state in the brain rises and
falls in
response to the electrochemical activation supplied by the reticular formation in the brainstem. |
|
3 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
67 |
|
For the brain
to be conscious, its nerve
cells must maintain a
certain level of electrochemical
activity. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
67 |
|
Overall brain
activation level
changes as
little as 10 percent (or at most 20
percent),
between waking
and sleep. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
68 |
|
Consciousness
operates within a very
narrow range of activation. Consciousness is exquisitely sensitive to even slight
changes in activation
level. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
68 |
|
A significant
amount of information processing occurs even when we are completely unaware of it
as we sleep. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
68 |
|
Spontaneously high
level of activation
during sleeping and dreaming; such processing is not only automatic but
potentially self-organizing and autocreative. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
68 |
|
Reticular formation works on the conscious mind not only by changing the level of
activation but also by modulating the neural inputs and outputs. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
69 |
|
Reticular formation, like a pair of sausages, occupies the central
core on each side of
the brainstem as it ascends from the medulla upward through the pons and midbrain to the hypothalamus. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
69 |
|
Reticular formation begins in the medulla, just above the level of the spinal cord. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
69 |
|
A particularly cogent example of
the function of the reticular formation is the coordination of eye
position,
which involves visual processing centers of the upper brain and spinal
circuits mediating head
and body position. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
69 |
|
Medulla programs many autonomic functions essential to consciousness as well as
organizing posture and controlling
head and neck position. [Stereotyped motor programs] [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
70 |
|
Pons and midbrain are the very center of the reticular
system because they so clearly coordinate activation of the higher
brain structures. [Stereotyped motor
programs] [FAPs] |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
70 |
|
Neural rhythms
-- Synchrony |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
70 |
|
When the activation level of the brainstem falls, even a little, the thalamocortical circuits begin to oscillate. This kind of synchrony contributes to the global loss of consciousness
that occurs in NREM
sleep. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
71 |
|
Oscillations
of the thalamocortical circuits that occur at sleep onset are robust and so highly synchronous that they cause the characteristic EEG pattern of slow wave sleep. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
71 |
|
Visual thalamus may be triggered
automatically in REM
sleep,
accounting in part for the detail of dream
consciousness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
72 |
|
Neural oscillations unify the brain's
disparate components. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
72 |
|
Reticular formation emerges as the coordinator, internal communicator,
and unifier of activity in
the modular brain. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
73 |
|
Consciousness
is determined by the neurochemical modulatory
systems of the brainstem
core. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
73 |
|
Neurochemical modulatory systems confer a second kind of unity on the brain, a metabolic one,
which complements electrical synchrony and
activation. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
73 |
|
Two main classes of modulatory neurons: (1) aminergic and (2) cholinergic systems. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
Cholinergic neurons are active in REM sleep. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
Two forms of consciousness: (1) waking and (2) dreaming. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
Memories
are ultimately encoded
as proteins in the synapses. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
In waking, the whole brain is primed to capture data. In sleep, the data might be differentially processed. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
Multiplicity
of neuromodulatory subsystems: aminergic (waking) side, at least
four different systems. (1) locus coeruleus-based noradrenergic system; (2) raphe nuclei-based serotonergic system; (3) midbrain-based dopaminergic system; (4) hypothalamus-based histaminergic system. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
74 |
|
During waking, all the neurons of the
noradrenergic, serotonergic, and histaminergic systems fire slowly and regularly. All of them fire more slowly in NREM
sleep. All of them stop
firing in REM sleep. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
75 |
|
Modulatory systems in the brain: (1) Noradrenergic, (2) Serotonergic, (3)
Dopaminergic, (4) Cholinergic - (diagram) |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
81 |
|
Process by which complex
functions arise when simple elements interact is called emergentism. |
|
6 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
82 |
|
Rapid reaction times in critical situations, like automobile operation, may be
life-saving. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
82 |
|
We may learn in an entirely
unconscious way, but because we can think abstractly, we can analyze our personal histories together with the
social context and thereby make deliberate choices. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
82 |
|
The resulting outcomes of our
choices can depend on entirely automatic behavior. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
84 |
|
Primary consciousness comprises sensation, perception, emotion, learning,
geographic orientation, instinct, primary intention; all of these can be
operationally defined in lower animals. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
85 |
|
In a developmental and
evolutionary sense, memory emerges out of learning. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
85 |
|
Learning may be completely
unconscious. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
87 |
|
Priming is
not just semantic,
but also sensory, and
often emotional. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
87 |
|
Neuronal networks are associatively connected and sequentially activated by one another. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
88 |
|
Even the simplest animals evince associative learning. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
88 |
|
Associative learning -- building
block of memory, of priming, and of word search must be a mechanism shared by neuronal
networks at all levels of phylogeny (evolution of species) and ontogeny
(individual development). |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
89 |
|
Comparing conscious components
in animals - (table) |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
89 |
|
Animal "mind" is that
set of brain functions that guides behavior so that innate, fully automatic,
genetically determined states (like hunger, fear and sexual arousal) can be
contextualized (integrated with ecological niches) by the individual. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
89 |
|
Learning is
the primary-level basis of memory; Purposefulness is the primary-level basis of volition. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
90 |
|
Snails and worms have neuronal
nets consisting of excitatory and inhibitory pathways that allow activation
levels to be controlled so as to enhance one or another output function. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
90 |
|
Ganglia of invertebrates contain neuromodulatory elements whose
chemicals subserve learning in the nets so that electrochemical activation
levels and input-output biases accurately reflect experience. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
90 |
|
Invertebrate ganglia contain hormone-producing
cells that, by secreting
chemicals that induce sexual and other consummatory behaviors, provide instinctual guidance to the organism. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
91 |
|
If an invertebrate ganglionic neuron is to keep a record of its experience,
the neurotransmitter serotonin must be released during its training-induced activation. No serotonin, no learning. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
91 |
|
Conscious state of waking, serotonin
is released, we perceive and can remember. In the conscious state of sleep (REM), serotonin is not released, we can perceive but not remember. No serotonin, no memory. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
91 |
|
Spinal cord,
much of the input-output processing and its modification
by experience can occur at local segmental levels. [Stereotyped motor
programs] [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
91 |
|
Reflexive machinery of the arm, hand, and finger is at the upper, cervical level of the spinal cord. [Stereotyped motor
programs] [FAPs] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
92 |
|
Reflex -- input becomes an output after passing through only one synapse. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
92 |
|
Consciousness
is our instantaneous awareness of the act of copying
information about the world, our bodies, and our
selves into our brains, and the integration of
those copies with all previous
copies.
[Edelman's 'remembered present'] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
92 |
|
Self -- the
resulting integration of perceptions, emotions, and memories. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
95 |
|
Young children are particularly vulnerable to dramatic shifts in temperature. Because their brain circuits are still developing, they are liable to develop seizures. |
|
3 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
95 |
|
Cognitive functions of orientation, memory, coherent speech, and stimulus-appropriate perception
are replaced in delirium by disorientation, amnesia, confabulation, and hallucination. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
96 |
|
Vision is a
symbolic process; no real pictures in the head, only neuronal patterns. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
97 |
|
Human infants are born with sensation, movement, instinct, emotion, and the
ability to learn. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
98 |
|
Mother
doesn't have to be conscious for her milk to flow in response to her baby's cry. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Active sleep of the newborn
infant is the primordial analog of adult REM sleep. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Neonatal REM sleep periods, stereotyped activation of
the brain, facilitating the development of the thalamocortical circuits necessary
for consciousness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Baby
associating the pleasure of eating with mommy or daddy's smile, could gradually build up into a confident sense of self as their consciousness gradually emerges. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Developmental psychologists postulate that consciousness emerges gradually during the second year of human life and culminates at about age 2 with the gaining of awareness of the self as an entity. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Infant at age 7-8 mo learns to control movement voluntarily.
Called "will"
by developmental
psychologists. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
99 |
|
Self arises
when sensations
associated with movement come to be taken as causes of the movement. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
100 |
|
Mature human thought and consciousness have both motoric and causal aspects. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
100 |
|
Recognition
memory is evident at 8
months of age. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
100 |
|
Retrieval
memory and inference allow a 14 month old to detect logical connection between past and present. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
100 |
|
Prefrontal
cortex is the seat of working
memory and
strategically willed action. Five functions of
self-aware, deliberate consciousness are integrated. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
100 |
|
REM sleep dreaming -- relative deactivation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is associated with: (1) loss of volition, (2) weakening of
working memory, (3) faulty inferential logic, (4) deterioration of
self-reflective awareness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
101 |
|
Neural origin of primary consciousness, thalamocortical system of the forebrain. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
104 |
|
Tucson I and II, conference held
in Tucson, AZ, 1994, 1996, "Toward a Science of Consciousness", wild heterogeneity of ideas from
diverse sources. |
|
3 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
115 |
|
PET scanning
(positron emission
recorded by computerized tomography). |
|
11 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
115 |
|
Working memory neurons of the
premotor cortex, receive dopamine and serotonin; Every cortical neuron
involved in the representation of working memory is influenced by a variety of chemical modulators. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
117 |
|
Optimists are people who proceed
to action despite risk. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
118 |
|
Stroke damage to left frontal lobe are much more
likely to become depressed than damage to right forebrain. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
118 |
|
Our minds, like our bodies, are
right- or left-handed. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
118 |
|
Highly hypnotizable subjects. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
124 |
|
Roger Penrose
-- relevance of quantum theory to the understanding of consciousness. |
|
6 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
124 |
|
Stuart Hameroff -- understanding the mechanism by which general anesthetic agents ablate consciousness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
126 |
|
Microtubules in neurons - neurons contain circulatory system, proteins can be
transported from nucleus to axon terminal and back again; protein transport mechanisms are quite slow, as long as 24 hours. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
126 |
|
Microtubules
may alter neuron excitability significantly, deliver enzymes that can manufacture or degrade
neurotransmitters. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
131 |
|
Comparison of the modules of
consciousness during Waking, Sleeping, Dreaming - (table) |
|
5 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
132 |
|
Electron isolation in the hydrophobic pockets of
proteins in microtubules - Stuart Hameroff
hypothesizes to be the basis of anesthetic
unconsciousness. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
134 |
|
Dream emotions - fear, anxiety, surprise, elation levels; much more
consistently enhanced in dreams than in waking. |
|
2 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
134 |
|
Dream fear
reaches such nightmarish
intensity as to demand a terrified escape reaction strong
enough to interrupt our slumber, breaking through the motor inhibition of REM sleep. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
134 |
|
Know so little how the brain constructs narrative. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
135 |
|
Normal waking conscious state; aware of where we are, the date and approximate time, who is
present in our
surroundings, goal or direction of our behavior. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
135 |
|
Orientational instability during dream
consciousness is at the root of dream bizarreness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
135 |
|
Dream disorientation, blockade of external sensory
signals, deprives the brain of time, place and
person cues that constantly update waking
consciousness and
maintain orientation. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
135 |
|
Waking consciousness; we know, but we also know that we
know. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
135 |
|
Dream consciousness, almost always wrong about itself. In dreams, we mistakenly assume that we are awake. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
136 |
|
We always correctly assume it is we and not someone else who is having our dream consciousness. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
136 |
|
Directed thought of waking consciousness; manipulate ideas, solve problems, examine the logic of
propositions, analyze the accuracy of observations and assumptions. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
136 |
|
In dreams we are cognitively
adrift; no mooring to time, place, or person, no
self-awareness, no critical thought. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
137 |
|
Chemicals
responsible for the evanescent neurotransmission of stimulus
signals is different from those responsible for the more permanent storage of mnemonic records - Neuromodulators |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
137 |
|
Among the neuromodulators
crucial to memory: (1) norepinephrine (2) serotonin; both are conspicuously diminished
during dream consciousness. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
140 |
|
Waking
depends upon the active
suppression of tendency of thalamocortical circuits to go
into their oscillatory mode. |
|
3 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
140 |
|
Thalamus is
the final internal
gateway for external information entering the
cortex. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
140 |
|
External information fails to
enter consciousness when the thalamocortical system is deactivated. Strength
of thalamocortical oscillatory circuits swamp those representing the outside world via the sensory
pathways. |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Thalamocortical system - In its
activated state, information
is rapidly and efficiently processed. Information
can be either online data from the real world or data about the real world that are stored in the brain. |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
The world, the self, and the body are re-represented in the network
activation of the thalamocortical
system.
[Edelman's dynamic core] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Consciousness
is the images (and thoughts and feelings) that are represented in the activated neural networks. Where
does it all come together? Nowhere and everywhere, simultaneously and
always. [Edelman's dynamic core] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Consciousness
at any instant is simply the integrated product of the information represented in the activated thalamocortical networks
at that instant. That includes sense of self; awareness of body; and awareness of the world, be it real or fictive.
[Edelman's dynamic core] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Can define consciousness at any instant as the information that is then represented in
the working memory circuitry of the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex (DLPFC).
[Edelman's dynamic core]
[Fuster's perception-action
cycle] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Information of consciousness implicitly reflects
my past in its representation of me as the agent of my motoric actions
(volition), includes my reflective sense of strategy for those actions
(thought), and their motivational appropriateness (emotional context). [Edelman's dynamic core] [Fuster's
perception-action cycle] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
141 |
|
Are all three
components of consciousness (volition, reflection, emotion) simultaneously present in working memory, or does working memory multiplex its inputs so rapidly as to create a semblance of
wholeness? Don't know the answer yet. [Edelman's dynamic core] [Fuster's
perception-action cycle] |
|
0 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
142 |
|
Hypothesis: Consciousness is
both continuous (in the overall direction of its flow) and discontinuous (in
its sampling of the myriad eddy currents of the multifarious inputs). |
|
1 |
Hobson; Consciousness |
209 |
|
Manic depressive illness -- Vincent van Gogh. |
|
67 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|