Parasuraman,
editor; Attentive Brain |
|
|
Chapter |
Page |
|
Topic |
|
|
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
3 |
|
Three components of attention: (1) selection, (2) vigilance, (3) control. All three aspects maintain goal-directed
behavior in a context of multiple, competing distractions. |
|
|
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
5 |
|
Diversity of attentional
functions has been discussed since at least the time of William
James (1890). James distinguished between (1) sensory attention driven by environmental events and (2) voluntary attention to both external stimuli and to internal thoughts. |
|
2 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
Many functions of attention are
carried out by different, though interacting, neural systems in the brain. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
Many brain processes run automatically and are influenced by attention slightly or not at all. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
Automatic processes may summon attention, as in the case of the response to the sudden onset of a peripheral stimulus, but they can also operate outside
awareness. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
Attention
serves the goals of (1) accurate and speedy
perception and action, (2) maintenance of processing over time. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
An organism's
goals are determined not only by the environment
but by the organism's internal dispositions, both temporary and enduring. This is presumably what links attention to motivation and emotion. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
A critically important component of attention is selection, which is perhaps the
most widely studied area. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
Selectivity of processing is required because of the computational
limitations imposed by fully
parallel processing of all sources. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
6 |
|
The large
receptive field of neurons in higher perceptual processing areas
results in a computational limitation. The primate brain presumably evolved mechanisms of selective attention to cope with
that limitation. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
7 |
|
Vigilance—or
sustained attention—ensures
that goals are maintained over time. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
7 |
|
Attentional control -- information-processing activity may need to be temporarily stopped (to respond to
some other important activity) and then resumed; there may be other concurrent
activities; and the future course of all such
activities must be coordinated. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
8 |
|
Cognitive neuroscience represents the merger of cognitive
psychology and neuroscience. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
9 |
|
Broca's area
lies adjacent to the area of motor cortex that controls the vocal musculature, and Wernicke's area is situated close
to the primary auditory cortex. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
9 |
|
Language is
based on adaptations of brain areas in early
humans who were preliterate and for whom language was primarily
spoken (Broca's area) and heard (Wernicke's area), rather than written and read. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
11 |
|
Event-related brain potentials
(ERPs) have clearly shown that selective attention modulates early-latency
ERP components, both in the visual and in the auditory modality. |
|
2 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
11 |
|
Early selection occurs as early as about 50 ms
after stimulus onset and involves modulation of brain electrical activity in sensory-specific cortical areas in a manner consistent with a sensory
gain-control mechanism. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
12 |
|
Attention a
cause or an effect? Whether there exist (1)
attentional systems that are separate from other sensory and motor systems in
the brain, or whether (2) attention represents an emergent property of other
processing activities. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
12 |
|
Functional brain-imaging studies have provided clear evidence for attentional
effects in many parts
of the brain. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Attentive Brain |
12 |
|
Anterior cingulate gyrus of the frontal lobe plays a key role in attentional
control and is therefore one of the brain areas
that act as the causal source. (Posner, 1995) |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
22 |
|
Cortical Processing Streams for
Object Vision and Spatial Vision |
|
10 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
22 |
|
First principle of cortical visual organization: Two
major corticocortical pathways, each beginning with primary visual cortex.
(1) Ventral pathway
is directed into the inferior temporal cortex and is important for visual
object recognition, (what) an object is. (2) Dorsal
pathway is directed into the posterior parietal
cortex and is important for spatial perception, (where) an object is. |
|
0 |
Webster; Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
23 |
|
Second principle of cortical
organization: Cortical areas within a pathway are organized hierarchically. Projections from lower-order areas to higher-order areas originate mainly in layer III of cortex and terminate
predominantly in layer IV. 'Feedforward' projections. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
23 |
|
Projections
from higher-order areas
to lower-order areas
originate mainly in layers V and VI of cortex and terminate both above
and below layer IV, but not
in layer IV. 'Feedback' projections. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
23 |
|
Connections between areas at the same hierarchical
level. Terminals vary their laminar pattern from
one patch to another; terminals homogeneously
distributed across all layers. 'Intermediate'
projections. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
24 |
|
Average receptive field size increases as one progresses along visual pathways, consistent with
the notion that receptive fields of cells in later areas are built up from receptive fields of earlier areas. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
24 |
|
Two principles of cortical organization, (1) parallel processing pathways, and (2) hierarchical organization. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
24 |
|
Subcortical structures and their connectivity must be included in
any analysis of anatomical organization of cognitive function. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
25 |
|
Filtering
of irrelevant visual information is accomplished via selective
attention mechanisms. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Attention
functions as a mental spotlight, enhancing the processing of the illuminated item |
|
2 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Network model (Mesulam's) of attention in which several distinct cortical
regions interact, including posterior parietal cortex,
cingulate cortex, and frontal
cortex, all of which are influenced by the reticular activating system. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Parietal component provides an internal perceptual map of the external world. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Cingulate component regulates the spatial distribution of motivational valence. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Frontal component coordinates the motor programs for exploration, scanning, reaching, and fixating. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Reticular component (including noradrenergic, dopaminergic, and cholinergic ascending systems) provides the underlying
level of arousal. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
27 |
|
Cortical components within the attention network are heavily and reciprocally
interconnected and are
also connected with subcortical structures, including the superior
colliculus, which is connected to the frontal eye fields and to the parietal cortex, and the pulvinar and striatum, which are connected to
all three cortical regions in the network. |
|
0 |
Webster; Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
Cortical areas related to attention are reciprocally
interconnected not
only with each other but also with the inferior
temporal and orbitofrontal
cortex. This arrangement provides an anatomical
substrate for parallel processing of information. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
Only the parietal, cingulate, and frontal
areas appear to be critical for the organization of directed attention. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
Model of attention (Posner's) consists of a (1) posterior attention network, an (2) anterior attention network, and a (3) vigilance network. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
The posterior network involves the parietal cortex, the pulvinar, and the superior colliculus. These areas
bring attention to a location in space. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
The anterior attention network involves the anterior cingulate cortex and supplementary motor areas in the frontal cortex, which together exercise executive
control over voluntary behavior and thought processes. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
28 |
|
The vigilance network involves the locus coeruleus noradrenergic input to the cortex,
which is crucial for maintaining a state of alertness. |
|
0 |
Webster; Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
29 |
|
Attention
is subserved by a system of spatially map
structures that are revealed
by the neglect
syndrome following brain
damage. The system operates to enhance perceptual processing at attended locations and reduce perceptual processing at unattended locations. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
29 |
|
Extremely large neuronal
receptive fields exists at the highest level of the processing pathways. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
29 |
|
Single neurons within the inferior temporal
cortex, which is the last
station of the ventral
pathway, have a receptive
field size of about 25°, or virtually the entire visual field. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
29 |
|
To limit the
amount of information that is processed, a model of attention has been
proposed based on neural competition. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
29 |
|
At several points between input
and response, objects in the
visual field compete for limited processing capacity and control of behavior. This competition can be biased by both bottom-up neural mechanisms that separate figures from their backgrounds as well as by top-down mechanisms
that bias competition
in favor of objects relevant to current behavior. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
30 |
|
The bias for attention can be controlled by selection of spatial location
or by selection of object features. |
|
1 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
30 |
|
Attention
is not a high-speed spotlight that scans each item in the visual field. |
|
0 |
Webster;
Neuroanatomy of Visual Attention |
30 |
|
Attention
is an emergent property of
slow competitive interactions that work in parallel across the visual field. |
|
0 |
Marrocco;
Neurochemistry of Attention |
35 |
|
A number of different cell
systems may be responsible for the filtering and selection of information. |
|
5 |
Marrocco;
Neurochemistry of Attention |
35 |
|
Attentional operations appear to be distributed across several networks of
structures. |
|
0 |
Marrocco;
Neurochemistry of Attention |
35 |
|
The anterior
attentional network,
which includes the frontal and cingulate cortex and the basal ganglia, is active during target detection and sustained attention. |
|
0 |
Marrocco;
Neurochemistry of Attention |
35 |
|
The posterior
attentional network,
which includes the parietal and inferotemporal cortices, the superior
colliculus,
and the medial pulvinar, becomes active
during visuospatial attention tasks and during the selection of
objects in the visual
field. |
|
0 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
52 |
|
Discharges
associated with a single neuron often produce a rather consistent spike-like shape that differs significantly in its waveform from other nearby neurons. |
|
17 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
52 |
|
The variability in interspike intervals observed for many neurons in
various locations in the nervous system is
consistent with the presence of a random Poisson
process. |
|
0 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
52 |
|
Information
appears to be coded
within single neurons only
by the average rate of firing and not by the precise composition of the intervals between spikes. |
|
0 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
53 |
|
Cortical neurons receive around 5,000--10,000
synapses, of which about 85% are excitatory. If inputs
arrived randomly, their integration would result in a fairly regular output train. |
|
1 |
Motter;
Neurophysiology |
53 |
|
The effect of inhibition in cortical circuits is
not simply an antagonistic balancing act versus excitation. Inhibition acts as a trimming damper on
the explosive growth of positive
feedback gain of excitatory cortical circuits. Relatively small amounts of inhibition provided at the correct time can shape the amplification of information. |
|
0 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
53 |
|
Modeling studies emphasize the
necessity of considering the collective action of neural assemblies in information
processing. |
|
0 |
Motter; Neurophysiology |
53 |
|
Synaptic integration at most cortical synapses can be regulated by slow and
long-acting neuromodulators. Many of these neuromodulator systems originate in
areas of the basal
forebrain and
brainstem, areas that exert major state controls
over waking and attentive behavior. |
|
0 |
Luck; ERPs |
71 |
|
Event-related potentials (ERPs) are electrophysiological responses that arise during sensory,
cognitive, and motor
processing and can be
recorded noninvasively from normal human subjects. |
|
18 |
Luck;
ERPs |
73 |
|
If multiple
electrodes are used to measure
activity at many
scalp sites, the distribution of voltage over the scalp can be
used as an index of the neuroanatomical loci of the neural and cognitive
processes. |
|
2 |
Luck;
ERPs |
73 |
|
ERPs can be
used to assess the effects of attention on sensory and cognitive processing, providing a precise index of the timing of attentional processes and a somewhat
less precise index of the neural structures. |
|
0 |
Luck;
ERPs |
73 |
|
ERPs
typically arise as a result of the postsynaptic potentials that are created when neurotransmitters bind with receptors on postsynaptic neurons. |
|
0 |
Luck; ERPs |
81 |
|
Attention
operates at an early
stage of processing. |
|
8 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
96 |
|
First PET
scanner for brain
studies with built in the mid-1970s at Washington University. |
|
15 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
104 |
|
Visual objects can be described as a combination of simpler visual features (color,
motion, orientation, texture, disparity, and location). |
|
8 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
113 |
|
A superior
frontal region, located near Broadman's area 6, is commonly activated for locational working memory. |
|
9 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
115 |
|
Anatomical, physiological, and imaging studies indicate that the anterior
cingulate region is heterogeneous and includes multiple
representations. |
|
2 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
116 |
|
PET has
become a major tool for
the exploration of human cognition. |
|
1 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
116 |
|
Attention
to feature or to objects amplifies relevant
information in
specialized processing regions of extrastriate visual regions. |
|
0 |
Corbetta; PET Visual Attention |
116 |
|
Regions in the superior parietal cortex select object locations for focal processing, biasing activity in ventral regions related to object processing. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
123 |
|
Functional MRI, (fMRI) requires no exposure to ionizing radiation, no injections of tracers, and no sampling of blood. Technical difficulties imposed by working in a powerful ambient magnetic field have been overcome. |
|
7 |
Haxby; fMRI |
123 |
|
MRI uses
the radio frequency (RF)
electromagnetic waves emitted by the nuclei of hydrogen atoms with single-proton nuclei to construct detailed images of the
brain and other organs. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
126 |
|
fMRI measures
are virtually instantaneous, making hemodynamic
responses the only factor that limits temporal resolution. |
|
3 |
Haxby; fMRI |
126 |
|
fMRI is not limited by radiation dose restrictions. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
126 |
|
Enough data from one individual can be collected via fMRI to perform massive signal
averaging, increasing sensitivity and precision sufficiently to obtain detailed
maps of responses in an
individual brain. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
126 |
|
An fMRI
experiment consists of a series of images obtained over a
period that typically lasts from one to 20
minutes. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
130 |
|
fMRI has
the capability to detect changes in neural activity over intervals as brief
as a few seconds in
the brain structures that are only 2 mm across. |
|
4 |
Haxby; fMRI |
132 |
|
Selective attention can modulate activity in multiple areas that comprise its processing pathway. |
|
2 |
Haxby; fMRI |
132 |
|
Ventral and dorsal extrastriate
cortex. Dissociation between the ventral object vision pathway and the dorsal spatial vision pathway. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
132 |
|
Suppression
of processing of irrelevant and potentially distracting
information. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
134 |
|
Motor skill learning: Size of the cortical patch activated in motor cortex
increased significantly. |
|
2 |
Haxby; fMRI |
134 |
|
Representation of the learned
sequence included additional cortical columns in motor cortex. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
134 |
|
Change in the functional
connectivity between regions. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
135 |
|
fMRI studies of working memory. |
|
1 |
Haxby; fMRI |
135 |
|
Working memory for face identity: Three extrastriate regions and three prefrontal regions. Distributed neural system. |
|
0 |
Haxby; fMRI |
138 |
|
Attention
may modulate the
activity of cortical regions that process attended and unattended information, altering interconnections between
regions. |
|
3 |
Haxby; fMRI |
138 |
|
Selective attention to facial identity augmented interregional
correlations in the ventral
object vision pathway, whereas selective attention to the spatial location of faces
augmented interregional correlations in the dorsal object vision
pathway. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
146 |
|
Prefrontal cortex can exert an inhibitory, top-down influence on neural activity within primary sensory cortices. |
|
8 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
155 |
|
Damage to dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex produces impairments in sustained and phasic attention abilities, as well
as deficits in inhibitory control of external stimuli and internal cognitive processing. |
|
9 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
155 |
|
The prefrontal
lesion patient operates in a noisy internal environment deficient in the critical regulatory mechanisms
necessary for the maintenance of working memory,
executive control functions, and the use the strategies. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
Prefrontal cortex appears to have both inhibitory and facilitatory influences on sensory and cognitive processing. |
|
1 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
Early input
to primary sensory cortices is modulated
by an inhibitory, prefrontal control mechanisms. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
Later processing in association cortices is dependent on facilitory
prefrontal input. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
In addition to the sensory control mechanisms
subserving sustained attention and working memory, a prefrontal-hippocampal network is selectively engaged during processing of novel stimuli. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
Based on anatomical
connectivity, it is suggested that prefrontal cortex is ideally suited
to generate and evaluate internal
models of action. |
|
0 |
Swick
& Knight; Cortical Lesions and Attention |
156 |
|
In addition to its function and role in sustained
attention and working
memory, the prefrontal-hippocampal
system is crucial for detecting
changes in the environment and for discriminating between internally and externally derived models of the world. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
167 |
|
Majority of synaptic inputs to cortical cells (up to 90% or more of all links to excitatory synapses) is provided by other cortical cells and not by sensory input. |
|
11 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
167 |
|
Top-down influence in the
central nervous system. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
167 |
|
Importance of both bottom-up and top-down processes is exemplified
by Ullman's model of information flow in the visual cortex for object recognition. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
167 |
|
Attentional bottleneck that limits the total amount of
information made accessible to higher cognitive functions. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
168 |
|
A significant
reduction of complexity
is achieved if the recognition of an object (what is it?) can be separated from its localization (where is it?). |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
168 |
|
Decoupling
of task of recognition
and localization. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
168 |
|
Tight integration of the "what"
and "where" pathways is essential; at any time, it is necessary that the information
in the two pathways be unambiguously
correlated. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
169 |
|
Detection of elementary
features is most economically carried out by massively parallel processes early in the visual hierarchy. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
170 |
|
Time required for one shift
of attention is on the order of 30 - 50 ms. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
170 |
|
Much longer dwell times (than 30 - 50 ms) for the focus
of attention. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
170 |
|
Abruptly changing stimuli attract attention. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
171 |
|
Attention
as a gatekeeper for
memory and awareness, allowing only selected portions of the visual scene to enter working memory. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
171 |
|
Working memory may be thought of as providing intermediate storage for different
perceptual elements selected at different times. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
171 |
|
Another task of working memory is to establish spatial
relations between the selected
feature elements. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
174 |
|
Pulvinar nuclei of the thalamus play a significant role in the selection
of visual targets. |
|
3 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
175 |
|
In addition to the pulvinar, other significant areas
in the selection of
visual targets are the posterior
parietal cortex and the superior colliculus. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
175 |
|
In cell-gaited
architectures, cell populations that do not code for attended information are deactivated (suppressed). |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
178 |
|
The attentional
selection process works by marking (or
tagging) the selected stimuli, based on the
temporal fine structure
of neuronal spike trains. |
|
3 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
178 |
|
A tagged
stimulus not only survives the perceptual selection process but
will also lead to motor action. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
179 |
|
Cells in early
visual cortex (area V2) respond to visual stimuli with a spike train with a stochastic distribution of interspike intervals with an
appropriate, stimulus-dependent mean firing rate. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
179 |
|
Spike trains
of neurons whose receptive fields do not overlap with the focus of attention are distributed according to a homogeneous Poisson process. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
179 |
|
Spike trains
of cells with receptive fields within the focus of attention are distributed
according to a probabilistic distribution with a different
time structure, which is generated by modulatory influences from the saliency map. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
179 |
|
The modulated time structure of each spike train is realized as a periodic repetition with a frequency in the gamma, or 40 Hz range. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
179 |
|
No specific time structure is imposed on spike trains from any one single cell, but the modulation is manifest in the form of correlations (or synchronization) between spike trains of cells responding to an attended stimulus. |
|
0 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
180 |
|
Any
information-processing system with finite resources operating in the
real world requires an attentive mechanism, because the multitude of sensors would overwhelm its computational
capabilities if all
sensory inputs were processed
simultaneously. Efficient processing requires careful selection of the most relevant stimuli and suppression of all others. |
|
1 |
Niebur
& Koch; Computational Architectures for Attention |
181 |
|
Although bottom-up selection processing is essential
for attention, complexity arguments indicate that top-down processes are indispensable for the processing
of complex stimuli. |
|
1 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
190 |
|
Mechanisms of attention are related to energy constructs such as arousal and activation, which connote nonspecific neuronal excitability
deriving from the reticular formation and specific chemically-defined or thalamic systems that innervate the forebrain. |
|
9 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
190 |
|
Monoaminergic
or cholinergic systems are often correlated with higher
levels of arousal present in wakefulness or response to stress. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
190 |
|
Monoaminergic
or cholinergic systems can modulate the functioning of neuronal networks by adjusting the signal-to-noise
ratio of neural
signals in the forebrain
processing. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
191 |
|
Monoaminergic
neurotransmitter systems (noradrenergic, dopaminergic, serotoninergic) as well as cholinergetic (acetylcholine, ACh) system. |
|
1 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
199 |
|
Distinction
between sustained attention and vigilance, described as a state of readiness to detect and respond to unpredictable
and rare events. |
|
8 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
199 |
|
Noradrenergic locus ceruleus role in behavioral vigilance. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
202 |
|
Divided attention assumes that the brain is limited in its ability to monitor in parallel more than a finite number of inputs, and thus
requires a switching mechanism to enable effective time-sharing of activities. |
|
3 |
Robbins;
Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
205 |
|
Several attentional networks in the primate brain. Three regions of the primate brain appear to play unique
roles in attentional shifts: midbrain, including the superior colliculus (in the control of visual saccades and the movement of
attention); the lateral pulvinar of the thalamus, which mediates the engagement of attention at a novel attentional focus; and the posterior parietal cortex, which
probably mediates the disengagement of intention from a given location and which may account for some of the
deficits observed in the neglect syndromes. |
|
3 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
205 |
|
Neglect syndrome -- in which stimuli on one side of the sensory world are ignored.
The syndrome is generally produced by lesions to the contralateral brain either at the cortical or subcortical level. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
208 |
|
Primary acoustic
startle reflex is controlled by brainstem nuclei. |
|
3 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
211 |
|
A wealth of detailed theory
exists concerning attentional function in human subjects, derived from cognitive
psychology. |
|
3 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
211 |
|
Human paradigm suitable for
investigating different aspects of attention such as vigilance, divided attention, sustained attention, and selective attention. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
211 |
|
Projection
from the cholinergenic basal forebrain to the neocortex (primarily the anterior regions) plays a role
in attention. |
|
0 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
212 |
|
Striatal dopaminergic systems are involved in attentional processes such as readiness that operate at the output stage on mechanisms related
to response preparation. |
|
1 |
Robbins; Pharmacology, Arousal, Attention |
212 |
|
Control of attention at a thalamocortical
level. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
221 |
|
Vigilance
can be considered a basic primitive form of
attention. |
|
9 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
221 |
|
Brainstem
reticular formation, cortical activation or arousal system. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
221 |
|
Vigilance
as a state of readiness
to detect and respond
to certain small changes
occurring at random time intervals in the environment. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
222 |
|
It is easy to be briefly attentive to a conspicuous and predictable event,
such as a traffic light changing. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
222 |
|
It is
somewhat difficult to maintain
attention to some source of information to detect
the occurrence of infrequent, unpredictable
events over long
periods of time. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
222 |
|
Tasks requiring detection of transient signals over
long periods of time
are known as vigilance
or sustained attention
tasks. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
222 |
|
In vigilant
tasks, quality of
information is fragile
-- it declines over time
-- an outcome known as a vigilance detriment. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
222 |
|
Vigilance decrement. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
226 |
|
Neurobiology of vigilance. |
|
4 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Relate vigilance to arousal or alertness. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Arousal
refers to a variety of physiological and
behavioral changes characterized by a degree of excitation or energy mobilization. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Multiple subcortical
arousal systems and
fMRI studies of vigilance in humans. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Reticular formation and the intralaminar
thalamic nuclei to which it projects. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Reticular system -- multiple ascending pathways from subcortical nuclei, each associated with different neurotransmitters and neuromodulators that have different properties and different cortical innervation patterns. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
Four main projection systems have been identified as playing functional roles in arousal and attention: (1) cholinergic basal forebrain, (2) noradrenergic nucleus locus coerulus (LC), (3) dopaminergic median forebrain bundle, (4) serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
227 |
|
In addition to the intralaminar thalamic nuclei, two other thalamic nuclei, the reticular nucleus and the pulvinar, have been implicated in attention and arousal. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
228 |
|
Basal forebrain cholinergic system plays an important role in attention
and arousal processes |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
228 |
|
LC noradrenergic system role in cortical arousal. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
228 |
|
High-frequency, low-amplitude,
asynchronous cortical EEG, associated with the alert, waking state, appears to
reflect an increase in subcortical noradrenergic
activity with an efficient relay of subcortical afferent inputs to the cortex. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
228 |
|
Low-frequency, high-amplitude,
synchronous EEG, accompanying states of drowsiness and sleep, is associated
with decreased noradrenergic activity and a blockade of sensory-afferent thalamic input to the cortex. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
229 |
|
Alert attentiveness is associated with fast EEG beta (14-30 Hz) activity. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
229 |
|
Relaxed wakefulness in which attentiveness per se is not necessary is characterized by slower alpha (8-13 Hz) activity. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
229 |
|
Drowsiness
leads to even slower
theta (4-7 Hz) and delta (1-3 Hz) activity. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
235 |
|
Most perceptual
and cognitive processes are neurally mediated by multiple brain regions rather than
a single area. |
|
6 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
235 |
|
Posterior parietal, prefrontal and cingulate cortices are interconnected by axonal projections to adjacent rather
than to overlapping target sites in each area, without collateral inputs,
thus defining a parallel, reciprocally connected
network subserving spatial
attention. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
235 |
|
Attention functions are considered to be controlled by networks
of interconnected
brain regions. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
235 |
|
In a general architecture for attentional functions, parietal regions were postulated
to be associated with sensory representation of
the world and spatial
attention, the frontal cortex with motor representations and planning,
and the reticular formation with arousal and vigilance. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
236 |
|
Three interacting networks mediating different aspects of attention: (1) a posterior
attention system comprising parietal cortex, superior colliculus,
and pulvinar that is
concern was spatial attention; (2) anterior system centered on the anterior cingulate in the medial frontal lobe that mediates target detection and executive control; (3) a vigilance system consisting of the right frontal
lobe and brainstem
nuclei, principally the noradrenergic
locus coerulus (LC). |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
236 |
|
It has been suggested that the vigilance system is right
lateralized due to greater innervation of the right hemisphere by ascending noradrenergic pathways. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
237 |
|
Each of the models identifies
the brainstem reticular formation (or specific
subsystems of the reticular formation) as playing
important roles in vigilance. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
239 |
|
Long history of evidence
implicating the brainstem reticular formation and arousal. |
|
2 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
239 |
|
Damage to the
frontal lobes, especially right hemispheric lesions, impairs
cortical arousal. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
241 |
|
Prefrontal cortex is involved in arousal and in the overall level
of vigilance. |
|
2 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
241 |
|
Cingulate gyrus has been implicated in vigilance. Reduced activation of the anterior cingulate gyrus has been implicated in vigilance. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
242 |
|
Cingulate gyrus is the cortical area lying just
above the corpus callosum which is the nerve fiber tract surgically severed in split
brain patients. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
243 |
|
Although the right
frontal lobe may play
a dominant role in vigilance, integrated action of both
cerebral hemispheres as well as subcortical nuclei is required. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
243 |
|
Both hemispheres are involved in vigilance, but the right hemisphere
prenominates. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
244 |
|
Noradrenergic
brainstem reticular formation, intralamina thalamic nuclei, and the right prefrontal cortex are all involved in vigilance. |
|
1 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
244 |
|
A network
of brain regions -- including the LC noradrenergic system, the basal forebrain cholinergic system,
the intralaminar thalamic nuclei, and the right prefrontal cortex -- are involved in the initiation of the vigilance state and in cortical arousal. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
247 |
|
Neurobiology of vigilance. |
|
3 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
247 |
|
Relationship between vigilance
and brain systems regulating cortical arousal, and the identification of
brain regions that are involved in vigilance. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
247 |
|
Basal forebrain cholinergic arousal system in vigilance. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
249 |
|
Relationship
between vigilance and
brain systems that regulate cortical arousal. |
|
2 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
249 |
|
Vigilance
as distinct from arousal. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
249 |
|
How brain
regions that mediate vigilance are functionally interrelated. |
|
0 |
Parasuraman;
Varieties of Attention |
249 |
|
Noradrenergic
reticular formation, the right
prefrontal cortex, and the basal
forebrain cholinergic
system
are involved in the overall level of vigilance. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
257 |
|
Visual agnosias, defined in the classical sense as visual
object recognition problems, are more often seen
after damage to the temporal lobes, whereas visuospatial problems are more likely observed following damage to the parietal lobes. |
|
8 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
257 |
|
Areas along the ventral temporal pathway contain neurons that are responsive to object features such as color, brightness, and shape. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
257 |
|
Areas along the dorsal parietal pathway contain
neurons that are responsive to spatial features necessary for localization, perception of motion, and preparation for action. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
257 |
|
Modular system for visual processing, with different areas within each pathway responding to different features. The number of these specialized areas has been estimated at about 30. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
258 |
|
Features of
an object are bound to
each other to form a unique object. |
|
1 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
258 |
|
Each object is
bound to a location and
is separated from other objects either by its distinct
features or by its
relative location or by both. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
260 |
|
Unilateral visual neglect -- a person can lose visual
awareness of one half of the visual
world but maintain
awareness of the other
half. Patients with neglect do not respond to information that
is contralateral to
the side of the lesion. |
|
2 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
261 |
|
Neglect
does occur in retinal
coordinates, but it also
occurs in environmental
and object-centered coordinates as well. |
|
1 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
261 |
|
It is likely that some of the differences observed among patients with neglect are due to different functional contributions associated with the different
anatomical areas involved in spatial attention. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
261 |
|
Classical symptoms of neglect (spatial deficit for one half of the field), closely linked to functions of the parietal lobes. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
261 |
|
Acute stages after stroke, neglect is at its worst; may take up to six months for neurological problems to stabilize. By that time it is likely that clinical
symptoms of unilateral
neglect are gone. |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
274 |
|
Brains compute spatial information in order to perceive and attend to objects veridically. |
|
13 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
274 |
|
Neural response data suggests a high degree of neural interaction between areas of the cortex that are responsible for spatial perception (dorsal pathways) and other areas that code features and objects and perhaps implicit spatial
information (ventral
pathways). |
|
0 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
275 |
|
Binding or conjunctions of features that enter
awareness require a
high level of spatial representation. |
|
1 |
Robertson;
Attention and Parietal Function |
275 |
|
Visual awareness of more than one object may require the accurate
computation of space from a variety of spatial maps. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
299 |
|
A range of segmentation processes can influence selection, leading to a variety of senses in which visual attention may be object based. |
|
24 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
299 |
|
Different types of object based attention must be carefully distinguished in research studies of neural
substrates involved. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
301 |
|
Gestalt Grouping |
|
2 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
301 |
|
Gestaltists
noted that our visual experience is spontaneously organized into distinct groups and objects in a predictable manner. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
301 |
|
Regions that are closer, smaller, surrounded, higher in contrast, convex, or
symmetrical tend to become figural. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
301 |
|
Rarely, our competing perception factors yield ambiguous reversible organizations
such as Rubin's faces versus vase displays. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
301 |
|
Many of the Gestaltists'
phenomenal demonstrations may actually reflect
the operation of attentional mechanisms. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
302 |
|
Our ability to attend selectively should be
constrained by Gestalt grouping. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
303 |
|
When motion and proximity are pitted against each other, either
may predominate, depending on exact circumstances. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
309 |
|
Visual search
depends strongly on how the display is segmented. |
|
6 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
309 |
|
Attention
can also be restricted to items on one surface in stereoscopic depth. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
310 |
|
Items with sudden onset tended to be examined first during an apparently
serial scan, where is
items with other unique properties (e.g. color) apparently were not. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
310 |
|
Research studies concluded that visual transients capture attention. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
310 |
|
Motion per se
apparently does not capture attention, but motion that newly segments an object from a background that it was previously grouped together with
does lead to attention capture. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
310 |
|
Segmentation
strongly constraines feature
conjunction, so that miscombinations of presented features are less
likely between separate
objects or groups than within
them. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
311 |
|
Symmetry
and repetition
detection. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
312 |
|
Distractors
that are grouped with the target interfere more than do comparable distractors that are not so grouped. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
312 |
|
Visual search
is powerfully influenced by segmentation processes and the same applies to feature
integration. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
313 |
|
Auditory attention. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
313 |
|
Neglect is
a common deficit after
unilateral brain injury,
in which patients fail to acknowledge or respond appropriately to events toward the contralesion side of space. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
314 |
|
When copying
drawings, neglect
patients often omit
details on the
contralesional side. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
316 |
|
It has been argued that the visual system describes shapes
relative to their principal axis of elongation
or symmetry. |
|
2 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
316 |
|
Neglect can
apply to the left of segmented figures, with principal axes providing the divide between left and right sides for each shape. |
|
0 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
317 |
|
Extinction
is often regarded as a more primitive spatial phenomenon, in which patients
can detect isolated events toward either the contralesional or ipsilesional side, but miss contralesional stimuli presented simultaneously with a competing ipsilesional event. |
|
1 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
320 |
|
Lateral biases in attention that emerge following unilateral
brain injury in neglect
and extinction. |
|
3 |
Driver
& Baylis; Visual Object Segmentation |
320 |
|
Visual attention can be object based in a number of ways. |
|
0 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
327 |
|
Visual attention modulates neural responses in many parts of visual cortex. |
|
7 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
328 |
|
Visual attention is often started with the closely
related paradigms of visual
search and visual texture segregation. |
|
1 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
328 |
|
In a visual
scene containing many
discrete stimuli, there may be certain stimuli that are readily detected among the others
(this phenomenon is called pop-out or parallel search). |
|
0 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
329 |
|
The visual
world is multiply
represented by approximately
30 visual cortical areas, and the neurons in these areas encode a wide variety of visual information. |
|
1 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
332 |
|
Visual performance is strongly influenced by attention. |
|
3 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
346 |
|
The existence of serial and parallel attentional processes (i.e. focal attention and pop-out) is well accepted. |
|
14 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
346 |
|
Stimuli
that pop-out usually
possess a locally unique feature that is not shared by surrounding stimuli. |
|
0 |
Braun; Divided Attention |
346 |
|
When several
stimuli are prominent, pop-out seems to be determined by global
competition among prominent
stimuli. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
353 |
|
Spatial Working Memory and Spatial Selective Attention |
|
7 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
Neural processes involved in
spatial attention (theory proposed by Mesulam). |
|
1 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
Attentional processes are mediated by the activation of a network that includes four primary brain regions. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(1) frontal
component (the dorsolateral, or, premotor,
prefrontal cortex) provides a spatial map for the coordination of exploratory
motor processes. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(2) posterior
parietal component coordinates the formation of a
spatial representation of extrapersonal space. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(3) processes of the anterior cingulate gyrus provide a
spatial map of motivational valence. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(4) a subcortical
reticular component modulates the overall arousal and vigilance necessary for attentional processing. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
Mesulam's network mediates two
qualitatively different types of attentional processing. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(1) Tonic regulation of the
overall threshold that stimuli must exceed in order to reach consciousness.
The reticular component is claimed to mediate this process. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
(2) Phasic selection (i.e.,
location-specific enhancement) of items that will receive attentional
resources from the multiple items that reach threshold. The frontal,
parietal, and cingulate components of the network are claimed to subserve
that type of attentional processing. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
354 |
|
Majority of neuroscientific
studies of, spatial attention
place emphasis on the frontal and parietal
components of attentional
processing. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
361 |
|
Hemineglect,
syndrome that is classically associated with impaired spatial attention. |
|
7 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
361 |
|
Attentional deficits are most prevalent in parietal
cortex. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
361 |
|
Attentional deficits and subcortical lesions -- data implicate posterior and
medial thalamic areas
as well as the basal ganglia in spatial attention. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
361 |
|
Distinct attentional
disorders result from lesions of the inferior and superior areas of parietal cortex. |
|
0 |
Awh; Spatial Working Memory |
361 |
|
Right inferior parietal lesions are the most frequent correlate of hemineglect, which may be defined
as the failure to explore the contralesional side of space. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
381 |
|
Attention
and language; two of
the most widely studied aspects of human cognitive
skills. Basic elements of cognition. |
|
20 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
381 |
|
Seminal work on attention in the 1950s. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
383 |
|
Diversity
and newness of many
of the cognitive neuroscience techniques; frequent cases of inconsistent
findings. |
|
2 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
384 |
|
Uncertainty about localization of language in the brain. |
|
1 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
384 |
|
Language competes with
other processes, for
access to a limited-capacity executive attentional system. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
385 |
|
Subcortical activating systems in the thalamus, with its rich and diverse
connectivity to the cortex, have been considered a
likely site for selective engagement of specific cortical functions. |
|
1 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
387 |
|
Imaging studies support the
classic view of two left hemisphere regions being particularly important in the representation,
maintenance, and production of phonological codes for language: the posterior portion of the left superior temporal gyrus, or Wernicke's area, and the dorsolateral
portion of the left
inferior frontal gyrus, or Broca's area. |
|
2 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
388 |
|
Broca's area
was active in tasks
requiring discrimination of consonant sequences but not of vowels. |
|
1 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
388 |
|
Ability to represent the phonemic or orthographical pattern that corresponds to a word is the means of communication; |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
388 |
|
The goal of language is to communicate information -- and the semantic representation of the concepts that a word
stands for is the heart
of language. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
388 |
|
Lexical-semantic network. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
388 |
|
Neural convergence
zones. |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
389 |
|
Localization of Semantic Knowledge. |
|
1 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
389 |
|
Neural systems that represent the meaning of
words have proved particularly challenging to isolate anatomically, which has led
some to conclude that semantic knowledge is by its nature diffusely
represented, probably by a sort of distributed
processing systems |
|
0 |
Fischler;
Attention and Language |
389 |
|
Cell assemblies representing knowledge may be part of a distributed
network. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
401 |
|
A specific model of higher-level control to specify when executive attention is a necessary aspect of information
processing. |
|
12 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
402 |
|
First level of control operates via contention
scheduling, which uses schemas to coordinate well learned behaviors and thoughts. |
|
1 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
402 |
|
Once a schema is selected, it remains active until it reaches its goal or it is inhibited by a competitive schema or by higher-level
control. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
402 |
|
The contentions-scheduling mechanism corresponds to a routine
selection. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
402 |
|
When the situation is novel on highly competitive (i.e. when it requires executive control), a supervisory attentional system
intervenes and provides additional
inhibition or activation to the appropriate schema for this situation. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
We suggest that executive control operations are dissociable from other cognitive operations in a
task. |
|
1 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Contention scheduling works via local inhibition of competing schemas. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Visual sub-processing systems perform selection via a local competition in which the receptive fields are viewed as critical resources for which stimuli are competing. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Competition
for these sub-processing systems is resolved by inhibition in the local neural circuit. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Competition
can be biased by a top-down mechanism that selects objects that are important to the current behavior all goal. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Preferential increases of neuronal activity for selected features or locations. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Scalp electrical recording has demonstrated both increases of electrical activity from selected events and decreases from competing events. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
403 |
|
Like the supervisory
system mechanism, competition for control of behavior appears to be resolved at local sites by the relative amplification of the selected
competitor. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
405 |
|
Many studies involving the detection of targets or the resolution of conflict among targets
have found activation
in the anterior cingulate. |
|
2 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
405 |
|
Stroop task (Stroop, 1935) |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
406 |
|
Following
practice,
a schema will be formed that will trigger when the stimulus is presented, and the supervisory system will not be
necessary. |
|
1 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
406 |
|
Following extended practice, the anterior
cingulate and the left
lateral prefrontal cortex were inactive -- instead, there
was increased
activation in the anterior insula, activation similar
to that when subjects read
the words aloud. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
406 |
|
Anterior cingulate is active when the supervisory system is necessary for appropriate behavior, and the anterior
cingulate is inactive when the supervisory system should be inactive and the contention-scheduling
mechanisms are active. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
409 |
|
For a brain
area to perform a supervisory function, it must influence widely distributed parts
of the brain. |
|
3 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
409 |
|
Anatomical studies suggests that the anterior
cingulate, like many brain regions, has close contact with many other cortical areas. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
409 |
|
The cingulate's connections to the lateral frontal areas involved in word processing and to posterior parietal areas are involved in orienting are particularly strong. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
Anterior cingulate is active during tasks that require some thought and its activity is reduced or disappears
as tasks become routine. |
|
2 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
The anterior cingulate and other midline frontal areas are involved in
producing the local
amplification in neural
activity that accompanies top-down selection of items. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
It is well known from cognitive studies that a target word is processed more efficiently
following the presentation of the related prime word. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
A portion of the improvement
occurs automatically because the prime word activates a pathways shared with
the target. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
Another portion of the activation is top-down because the attention to the prime leads the subject to expect a
particular type of target. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
The cingulate in conjunction with other midline
areas is responsible for the top-down effects in that it
provides a boost in activation to items associated with the expectation. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
Anatomically, the cingulate is in contact with areas of the left lateral and posterior cortex that seemed to be
involved in understanding the meaning of a given target word. |
|
0 |
Posner; Executive Attention |
411 |
|
The time
course of activation of the cingulate (170 ms) and the left lateral frontal (220 ms) cortex during the generate use
task supports our speculation that attention interacts with the semantic activation pattern. |
|
0 |
Johnson; Developing Attentive Brain |
428 |
|
Most aspects of gross cortical structure, such as cell numbers and location, are in
place at the time of birth in the human infant. More detailed aspects of cortical structure such as synaptic density and glucose uptake, show prolonged postnatal development. |
|
17 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|