Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
Book |
Page |
|
Topic |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
17 |
|
In humans that cortex has grown
so large that it has been forced to become convoluted (folded), giving it its
famous cauliflower like appearance. (In contrast, the cortex of most other
mammals is smooth and flat for the most part, with few if any folds in the
surface.) |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
17 |
|
The brainstem is divided into
three lobes: medulla, ponds, and midbrain. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
The medulla and nuclei on the
floor of the ponds control important vital functions like breathing, blood
pressure, and body temperature. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
The cerebellum controls the fine
coordination of movements and is also involved in balance, gate, and posture. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
The cerebellum also receives
sensory feedback from muscles and joints receptors throughout the body. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
Mismatches between intended
action and the actual action can be detected and appropriate corrections can
be inserted into the outgoing motor signal. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
The thalamus receives its major
inputs from the sense organs and relieved relays them to the sensory cortex. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
The basal ganglia air are
cluster of structures that are concerned with the control of automatic
movements associated with complex volitional actions. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
18 |
|
Damage to cells in the basal
ganglia result in disorders like Parkinson's disease, in which the patient's
torso is stiff, his face is an expressionless mask, and he walks with a
characteristic shuffling gait. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
Excessive amounts of brain
chemical dopamine and the basal ganglia can lead to disorders known as
choreas which are characterized by uncontrollable movements that have a
superficial resemblance to dancing |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
Each cerebral hemisphere of its
cerebral cortex is subdivided into four lobes: occipital, temporal,
Parenteau, and frontal. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
Tthe occipital lobes are mainly
concerned with visual processing. They are subdivided into as many as 30
distinct processing regions. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
The temporal lobes a specialized
for hire perceptual functions, such as recognizing faces. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
The upper part of the left
temporal lobe contains a patch of cortex known as Wernicke's area. In humans
this area is seven times the size of the same area in chimpanzees. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
Wernicke's area function is the
comprehensive comprehension of meaning and the semantic aspects of language. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
19 |
|
The parietal lobes are primarily
involved in processing touch, muscle, and joint information from the body and
combining it with vision, hearing, and balance to give you a rich
"multi-media" understanding of your self and the world around you. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
22 |
|
Mirror neurons fire
not only when you perform an action, but also when you watch someone
else perform the same action. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
22 |
|
What mirror
neurons do is effectively allow you to empathize with another person and read their intentions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
22 |
|
Usually your mirror
neurons are reasonably
accurate guesses of others'
intentions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
22 |
|
Mirror neurons are also seen in apes, but only in humans have these neurons developed to the point of
being able to model aspect of others' minds rather than merely
their actions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
23 |
|
Mirror neurons may well be central to social
learning, imitation, and cultural transmission of skills and
attitudes. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
23 |
|
By hyperdeveloping the mirror neuron system, evolution in effect turned culture into the new genome. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
23 |
|
Culture
became a significant new source of evolutionary pressure, which helped select for brains that had even
better mirror neuron systems and imitative learning associated with them. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
38 |
|
Laughter --
and its cognitive companion, humor -- is a universal trait present in all cultures. |
|
15 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
38 |
|
Why and how humor
evolved is a mystery. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
38 |
|
In any joke or humorous incident, you narrate a story step-by-step, leading the listener along the garden path of expectation, and then introduce an unexpected twist, a punch line, the comprehension of which requires a complete
reinterpretation of the preceding events. But that's not
enough.
The extra key ingredient is that the new interpretation must be inconsequential. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
39 |
|
Laughter
may be nature's 'false alarm' signal. By laughing you inform people in the vicinity not to waste
their resources rushing
to an unfortunate victim's aid. It is nature's "all's okay" signal. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
39 |
|
The sensation
of pain is initially processed in a small structure called the insula ("island"), which is folded
deep beneath the temporal
lobe on side of the
brain. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
39 |
|
From the insular the pain information is then relayed to the anterior cingulate in the frontal lobes. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
39 |
|
It is in the anterior
cingulate that you feel the actual unpleasantness -- the agony and awfulness of pain -- along with an expectation
of danger. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
39 |
|
Humans have
two key ingredients
for laughter -- a palpable and imminent indication
that alarm is warranted (from the insula) followed by a "no-big-deal" follow-up. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
41 |
|
Carnivores
and herbivores
probably have fewer than a dozen visual areas and no color vision. |
|
2 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
41 |
|
Humans have
as many as 30 visual areas instead of a mere dozen. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
42 |
|
The shift
in diet of our shrew-like ancestors from tiny nocturnal insects to red, yellow, and blue fruits, as well as to leaves whose nutritional value was color-coded into various shades of green, brown, and yellow, propelled the
emergence of a sophisticated system for color vision. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
62 |
|
Visual information from the retina gets to the brain via two
pathways.(diagram) |
|
20 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
62 |
|
The old pathway relays through the superior colliculus, arriving
eventually in the parietal lobe. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
62 |
|
The new visual pathway goes via the lateral genetically nucleus (LGN) to the visual cortex and then splits once again into the "how" and "what" streams. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
62 |
|
The old pathway enables us to orient toward objects and track them with our eyes and heads. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The new pathway, which is highly
developed in humans
and in primates
generally, allows sophisticated analysis and recognition of complex
visual scenes and objects. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The new pathway projects from
the retina to V1, the first and largest of our cortical visual maps, and from
their splits into two subpathways, or streams -- the "where" stream
and "what" strain. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The "where" stream is concerned with the relationship among visual objects in space. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The what stream is concerned
with the relationships of features within visual objects themselves. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The where stream's function
overlaps to some extent with that of the old pathway, but it mediates much
more sophisticated aspects of spatial vision -- determining the overall
spatial layout of the visual scene rather than just the location of an
object. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
The "where"
stream projects to the parietal
lobe and has strong
links to the motor
system. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
Most of these computations are unconscious and highly automated. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
A blindsight patient suffered substantial damage to his left visual cortex -- the origin for both the "where" and "what" streams. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
63 |
|
Blindsight
discovered in Oxford
by Larry Weizkrantz
in the late 1970s. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
64 |
|
The "what"
pathway projects from
V1 to the fusiform gyrus, an area mainly
performing the
classification of objects. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
64 |
|
Some aspects
of meaning are
probably fed back
from higher centers
to the fusiform. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
64 |
|
As the ventral
pathway precedes past
the fusiform to other parts of the temporal lobes, it evokes not only the name of the thing but a penumbra of associated memories and facts about it -- broadly
speaking the semantics, or meaning, of an object. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
64 |
|
The semantic retrieval process
involves widespread activation of the temporal lobes, but it seems to center
on a handful of bottlenecks that include Wernicke's language area and the
inferior parietal lobule (IPL), which is involved in quintessentially human
abilities such as naming, reading, writing, and arithmetic. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
Once the meaning is extracted, the neural
messages are relayed
to the amygdala, evoking feelings about what (or whom) you are seeing. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
A third pathway carrying
biologically salient stimuli such as eyes, food, facial expressions, and
animate motion passes from the fusiform gyrus through an area of the temporal
lobe called the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and then straight to the
amygdala. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
The third
pathway bypasses high-level object perception and shunts
quickly to the amygdala. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
The third
pathway is a shortcut, probably evolved to promote fast reaction to the high-value situations, whether innate or learned. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
The amygdala works in conjunction with past
stored memories and other
structures in the limbic system to gauge the emotional significance. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
For an intense feeling, the
signals from the amygdala cascade into the hypothalamus, which not only
orchestrates the release of hormones but also activates the autonomic nervous
system to prepare for appropriate action. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
Medical students use the mnemonic of the
"four F's" to remember feeding,
fighting, fleeing, or wooing. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
The autonomic responses include
all of the physiological signs of strong emotion such as increased heart
rate, rapid shallow breathing, and sweating. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
65 |
|
The amygdala is also connected
with the frontal lobes, which adds subtle flavors of the 4F cocktail of
primal emotions, so that you have not just anger, lust, and fear, but also
arrogance, pride, caution, admiration, magnanimity, etc. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
66 |
|
Segmentation, the function
involved in knowing which fragments of a visual scene belong together to
constitute a single object, is a critical prelude to object recognition in
the "what" stream. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
67 |
|
Color vision area
V4 lies in the same brain region -- the fusiform gyrus -- as the face recognition area. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
68 |
|
Emotionally responding to the recognition of objects or
faces is the very last step in the chain
of events that we call
perception. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
69 |
|
Strong sexual
attraction to his mother when he was a baby -- the so-called Oedipus
complex. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
70 |
|
Part of the continuous autonomic
reactions to experience is microsweating -- your whole body, including your palms, become damper or dryer in proportion to the level of emotional
arousal. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
70 |
|
measure emotional reaction to
things by simply monitoring the degree of micro selecting. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
70 |
|
tape being too passive
electrodes to the skin and routing them through a ohmmeter. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
70 |
|
monitor a galvanic skin response
(GSR), the moment to moment fluctuations and the electrical resistance of the
skin. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
94 |
|
The number
area and V4 are right next to each other in the fusiform
gyrus. |
|
24 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
94 |
|
The most
common type of synesthesia is the number-color type, and the number and color areas are immediate neighbors in the brain. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
94 |
|
Since the 19th
century
a debate has raged between phrenology -- the notion that different
functions are sharply
localized in different brain areas -- versus holism, which holds that functions are emergent properties of the entire brain whose parts are in constant interaction. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
95 |
|
Even high-level perception such
as tools or vegetables or fruits -- which border on being concepts rather
than mere perceptions -- can be lost selectively depending on the particular
small region of the brain that is damaged by stroke or accident. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
95 |
|
The brain's
specialized regions are organized as hierarchies. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
95 |
|
In the brain's
hierarchical system, each higher
level carries out more
sophisticated tasks, but there is an enormous amount of feedback and crosstalk. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
95 |
|
Numerical computation seems to
occur in stages -- an early stage in the fusiform gyrus where the actual
shapes of numbers are represented, and a later stage in the angular gyrus
concerned with numerical concepts such as ordinality (sequence) and cardinality
(quantity). |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
98 |
|
The insula maps internal feelings from your body. |
|
3 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
98 |
|
The insula receives continuous streams of sensation from receptor cells in your heart, lungs, liver, viscera, bones, joints, ligaments,
fascia, and muscles, as well as from specialized receptors in your skin that sense heat, cold, pain, sensual touch,
and perhaps tickle and itch. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
98 |
|
The insula uses its input streams of
information to represent
how you feel
in relation to the outside world and your immediate environment. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
98 |
|
As a central
player in your emotional
life,
your insula
sends signals to and receives
signals from other emotional centers in your brain including the amygdala, the autonomic nervous system (powered by the hypothalamus), and the orbitofrontal cortex, which is
involved in nuanced emotional judgments. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
98 |
|
The insula receives strong taste input from the tongue. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
101 |
|
The fusiform gyrus represents not only
shapes like letters of the alphabet but faces as well. |
|
3 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
102 |
|
Neurally linking color with emotion was probably
initially to attract us to ripe fruit and/or tender new shoots and
leaves. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
102 |
|
The linked effects of color and
emotion probably arise through interactions between the insula and higher
brain regions devoted to color. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
103 |
|
The musician
Listz may have seen
colors where there were only tones. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
104 |
|
One thing that poets and novelists have in common is that
they are especially good at using metaphor. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
106 |
|
Puns are
based on superficial associations. |
|
2 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
106 |
|
Schizophrenics, who have miswired brains, are terrible at interpreting metaphors and proverbs, yet according to clinical folklore, they are very good at puns. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
106 |
|
Both metaphors and puns involve linking seemingly unrelated concepts. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
106 |
|
Puns are
actually the opposite of metaphor. A metaphor exploits a surface-level
similarity to reveal a deep hidden connection. A pun is a surface-level similarity that masquerades as a deep one -- hence its comic
appeal. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
107 |
|
Relatively high incidence of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in humans. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
107 |
|
The
retention of genes for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in the human population may have advantages in boosting creativity,
intelligence, or subtle
social-emotional faculties. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
117 |
|
Mirror neurons may have played a pivotal role in humans becoming the one and only species
that veritably
lives and breathes culture. |
|
10 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
117 |
|
Culture
consists of massive collections of complex skills and knowledge, which are transferred from person to person
through two core mediums: language and imitation. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
The ability to save the world
from another person's vantage point is essential for constructing a mental
model of another person's complex thoughts and intentions in order to predict
and manipulate his behavior. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
A theory of mind is unique to humans. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
Certain aspects of language was
probably built partly on our facility for imitation. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
Darwin's theory
of evolution is one of the most important
scientific discoveries of all time. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
Unfortunately, Darwin's theory of evolution makes no provision for an afterlife. Consequently it has
provoked more acrimonious debate than any other topic in science. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
Richard Dawkins has stated that teaching intelligent design is little different from giving equal
status to the idea that the sun goes around the earth. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
The hominid
brain reach nearly its present
size, and perhaps even its present intellectual capacity,
about 300,000 years ago. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
118 |
|
Many of the attributes we regard
a uniquely human -- such as toolmaking, fire
building, art, music, and
perhaps even full-blown language -- appeared only much later, around 75,000 years ago. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
Crude Oldowan
tools -- made by just
a few blows to a core
stone to create an irregular
edge --
emerged 2.4 million years ago and were probably made by Homo habilis, whose brain
size was halfway
between that of chimps and modern humans. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
After 1 million years of
evolutionary stasis, aesthetically pleasing symmetrical tools began to
appear, which reflected a standardized production technique. These required
switching from a hard hammer to a soft, perhaps a wooden, hammer while the
tool is being made, so as to ensure a smooth rather than a jagged irregular
edge. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
The invention of stereotyped assembly-line tools --
sophisticated symmetrical
bifacial tools
that were hafted
to a handle -- took place only 200,000 years ago. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
What was the role
of tool use
in
shaping human cognition? |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
"Great leap" in mental
sophistication about 60,000
years ago --
widespread cave art, clothing, and constructed
dwellings appeared. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
Humans are
often called the "Machiavellian
primate,"
referring to our ability to predict other people's behavior and outsmart
them. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
119 |
|
Why are humans so good at
reading one another's intention? |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
How did language
evolve? |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
Unlike many
other human traits such as humor, art,
dancing, and music, the survival value of language is obvious -- it lets us communicate our thoughts and intentions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
The human vocal apparatus is vastly more sophisticated than any other
ape, but
without the correspondingly sophisticated language areas in the brain, such exquisite
articulatory equipment alone would be useless. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
Maybe our vocal
equipment and our remarkable ability to modulate our voice evolved mainly for producing
emotional calls and musical
sounds during courtship in early primates. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
Is language mediated by a sophisticated and highly specialized mental language organ that is unique to humans as suggested
by MIT linguist Noam Chomsky? |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
120 |
|
Is there a
more primitive gestural communication system already in place that provided scaffolding for the emergence of vocal language? A major piece of
the solution comes from the discovery of
mirror neurons. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
121 |
|
In monkeys, mirror neurons enable that prediction a simple goal-directed actions of other monkeys. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
121 |
|
In humans, mirror neurons have become sophisticated enough to interpret even complex actions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
121 |
|
Mirror neurons also enable us to an imitate the movements of others, thereby setting the stage for cultural "inheritance" of skills developed and honed by
others. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
122 |
|
Mirror neurons may also enable you to mime the lip and tongue movements of others, which could provide the evolutionary basis for vertebral utterances. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
122 |
|
The mirror
neuron arguments do
not in any way negate the idea that there are specialized
brain areas for language in humans. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
One of the chief areas where mirror neurons abound in monkeys, the ventral
premotor area,
may be a precursor of our celebrated Broca's area, a brain center associated with the expressive aspects of human language. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
Language is
not confined to any single brain area, but the left
inferior parietal lobe is certainly one of the areas that are crucially involved, especially in the representation of word meaning. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
In the strange disorder call anosognosia, people seem unaware
of or deny their disability. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
Most people
with a right hemisphere stroke have complete
paralysis of the left side of their body and, as you might
expect, complain about it. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
About 5% of
patients with right
hemisphere stroke will
vehemently deny their paralysis, even though they
are mentally otherwise lucid and intelligent. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
123 |
|
President Woodrow Wilson, whose left
side was paralyzed by a stroke in
1919, insisted that he was perfectly fine. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
124 |
|
Neurons in the anterior
cingulate have long been known to respond to physical pain. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
124 |
|
Anytime you watch someone doing something, the neurons that
your brain would use to do the
same thing become active -- as if you yourself were doing it. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
125 |
|
Some degree
of leakage from the mirror neuron system can occur in
normal individuals. We feel ourselves unconsciously
flexing our muscles when watching
an athlete beginning to throw a football. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
126 |
|
Are mirror
neuron functions present innately, or learned, or perhaps a
little of both? |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
126 |
|
How are mirror
neurons wired up, and how do they perform their functions? |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
127 |
|
A newborn infant will often
protrude its tongue when watching its mother do it. The neural circuitry
involved must be hardwired and not based on associative learning. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
127 |
|
An infant's smile echoing its
mother's smile appears at about six weeks, but it can't be based on learning
since the infant can't see its own face. It has to be innate. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
Mirror neurons allow you to figure out someone
else's intentions. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
The ability to have a theory of
mind may exist in the great apes in rudimentary form, but humans are
exceptionally good at it. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
In addition to allowing us to
see the world from another person's visual vantage point, mirror neurons have
evolved further, enabling us to adopt the other person's conceptual vantage
point. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
We use metaphors like "I see
what you mean" or "Try
to see it from my point of view." |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
As a corollary to adopting the others point of view, you can also see
yourself as others see you -- an essential ingredient of self awareness. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
128 |
|
When we speak of someone being self-conscious, what we really mean
is that she is conscious of someone else being conscious of her. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
132 |
|
Most English
speakers have a vocabulary of about 10,000 words. |
|
4 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
132 |
|
Mirror neurons allow us to imitate. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
132 |
|
Miming may
have been the key step in
hominid evolution, resulting in our ability to transmit knowledge through example. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
138 |
|
The human's highly sophisticated theory of mind is one of the most
unique and powerful faculties of the human brain. |
|
6 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
138 |
|
Human theory of mind apparently
does not rely on general intelligence -- the rational intelligence used to
reason, draw inferences, combine facts, etc. -- but on a specialized set of
brain mechanisms that evolved to endow us with our equally important degree
of social intelligence. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
139 |
|
Perhaps autistic
children's profound deficits in social interactions stem from their theory of mind circuitry being somehow compromised. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
139 |
|
Many brain
imaging studies have
been conducted on children with autism. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
139 |
|
Children with autism have larger
brains within large ventricles. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
139 |
|
Symptoms
typical of autism
(e.g. lack of empathy and social skills) are never seen in cerebellar disease. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
140 |
|
Perhaps the main
cause of autism is a dysfunctional mirror-neuron system. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
163 |
|
Only humans, as far as we know, can use metaphor and analogy. |
|
23 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
163 |
|
Flexible, recursive syntax is found only in human language. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
163 |
|
Two topics
in brain research always
seem to attract geniuses and crackpots. One is consciousness and the other is the
question of how language evolved. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
164 |
|
Richard Dawkins has pointed out they are numerous
creatures in nature with eyes at
all stages of complexity. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
165 |
|
Simplest
possible light sensing mechanism -- a patch of light sensitive cells on the outer
skin. |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
165 |
|
Darwin's
contemporary Alfred Russell Wallace, who independently discovered the principle of natural selection. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
165 |
|
Founding father of modern linguistic science, Noam Chomsky. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
165 |
|
A good example of emergence is
the taste of salt, formed by combining the pungent, greenish, poisonous gas
chlorine with the shiny, light metal sodium. Neither of these elements has
anything salt-like about it. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
175 |
|
Performing the translation between visual appearance, sound representation in auditory cortex, and sequences of muscle twitches in Broca's area almost certainly
involve the activation of circuits would mirror-neuron-like properties. The inferior
parietal lobules (IPL), rich in mirror neurons, is ideally suited
for this role. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
176 |
|
Semantics
-- roughly speaking,
the meaning of a sentence. |
|
11 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
176 |
|
Although we know that Wernicke's area and parts of the temporo-parieto-occipital (TPO) junction, including the angular gyrus, are critically involved in semantics, we have no idea how neurons in these areas actually do their job. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
176 |
|
The manner in which neural
circuitry embodies meaning is one of the great unsolved mysteries of
neuroscience. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
178 |
|
The IPL (including the angular gyrus) -- strategically located between the touch, vision, and hearing parts of the brain --
evolved originally for cross modal abstraction. |
|
2 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
178 |
|
The upper
part of the IPL, the supramarginal gyrus, is also unique to humans, and is directly involved in the production,
comprehension, and imitation of complex skills. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
181 |
|
Syntax --
the aspect of language
that is most unequivocally human. |
|
3 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
181 |
|
Syntactic structure gives human language its enormous range and flexibility. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
181 |
|
Exaptation
principle -- the notion that adaptation to one specific function becomes assimilated
into another, entirely
different function. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
183 |
|
Two key human
abilities -- language and abstraction. |
|
2 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
183 |
|
Link between language and sequential thinking, or reasoning in logical steps. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
183 |
|
Thinking
involves, among other things, the ability to engage in open-ended symbol manipulation in
your brain following certain rules. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
188 |
|
The one aspect that makes the deep grammatical structure in humans unique is recursive embedding. |
|
5 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
188 |
|
When people say that deep structure and syntactic
organization are normal in Wernicke's aphasia, they are usually referring to
the more obvious aspects, such as the ability to generate a fully formed
sentence employing nouns, prepositions, and conjunctions but carrying no
meaningful content. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
Wernick's aphasia example, "John and Mary went
to the joyful bank and paid hat." |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
The speech output of Wernicke's
aphasics isn't entirely normal even in its syntactic structure. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
In the speech of Wernicke's aphasics, in addition to the absence of meaning, the most striking an obvious loss is recursive embedding. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
Wernicke's aphasics speak in loosely
strung together phrases using conjunctions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
Recursion
may turn out to be a property of Wernicke's area, and indeed may be a general property of many brain functions. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
189 |
|
Language capacity evolved through natural selection over 200,000
years. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
All mammals have three tiny bones -- malleus, stapes, and incus -- inside the middle ear (mammals have them but they are reptile ancestors don't). |
|
1 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
Comparative anatomists,
embryologists, and paleontologists discovered that the middle
ear bones
evolved from the back of
the jawbone of the reptile. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
The mammalian
jaw has a single bone, the mandible, whereas the reptilian ancestors have three. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
Reptiles,
unlike mammals,
frequently consume enormous prey rather than frequent small meals. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
The reptile
jaw is used exclusively
for swallowing, not chewing, and due to reptiles' slow metabolic rate, the unchewed
food in the stomach can take weeks to break down and
digest. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
The reptile kind of eating requires a large, flexible, multihinged jaw. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
Reptiles
lie low on the ground with their limbs
sprawled outward, swinging their neck and head close to the
ground while they sniff for prey. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
For reptiles, the three bones of the jaw lying on the ground allowed reptiles to transmit sounds made by other animals' nearby footsteps to the vicinity of the ear. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
190 |
|
As reptiles evolved into mammals, they raised themselves up from a sprawling
position, which allowed two of the three jaw bones to become progressively assimilated into the middle ear, being taken over entirely for hearing
airborne sounds and giving up the chewing
function altogether. |
|
0 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
192 |
|
How does the human brain respond
to beauty? |
|
2 |
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
200 |
|
Gestalt psychologists were good
at pointing out laws of perception. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
201 |
|
the log grouping was discovered
by Gestalt psychologists around the turn of the 20th century. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
205 |
|
looking at the big object of
which only fragments of visible. Many cells fire and parallel to signal the
different fragments. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
205 |
|
Synchrony tells higher brain
centers that the fragments belong to a single object. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
205 |
|
synchrony of Liles the spike
trains to be encoded in such a way that a coherent output emerges, which is
relayed to the emotional core of the brain. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
206 |
|
there are back projections from
the amygdala and the and of the brain structures such as the nucleus
accumbens to almost every visual area in the hierarchy of visual processing. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
206 |
|
the the shift effect relates to
how your brain responds to exaggerated stimuli. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
209 |
|
The relevance of the peak shift
law to caricatures and to the human body is obvious. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
212 |
|
of the 30 or so visual
processing areas in the human brain, a few – especially V4 – are devoted
primarily to color. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
213 |
|
The principal of ultranormal
stimuli may be relevant not just to art but to the quirks of aesthetic
preference such as whom you are struck attracted to. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
236 |
|
metaphor and language is well
known, but it's not widely appreciated that it's also used extensively in
visual art. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
236 |
|
multiple layers of metaphor and
meaning in sculpture. |
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
Ramachandran;
Tell-Tale Brain |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|